11
Genetic adaptation to captivity can occur in a single generation Mark R. Christie a,1 , Melanie L. Marine a , Rod A. French b , and Michael S. Blouin a a Department of Zoology, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR 97331-2914; and b Oregon Department of Fish and Wildlife, The Dalles, OR 97058-4364 Edited by Fred W. Allendorf, University of Montana, Missoula, MT, and accepted by the Editorial Board November 11, 2011 (received for review July 14, 2011) Captive breeding programs are widely used for the conservation and restoration of threatened and endangered species. Neverthe- less, captive-born individuals frequently have reduced tness when reintroduced into the wild. The mechanism for these tness de- clines has remained elusive, but hypotheses include environmental effects of captive rearing, inbreeding among close relatives, re- laxed natural selection, and unintentional domestication selection (adaptation to captivity). We used a multigenerational pedigree analysis to demonstrate that domestication selection can explain the precipitous decline in tness observed in hatchery steelhead released into the Hood River in Oregon. After returning from the ocean, wild-born and rst-generation hatchery sh were used as broodstock in the hatchery, and their offspring were released into the wild as smolts. First- genera tion hat chery sh had near ly double the lifetime reproductive success (measured as the number of returning adult offspring) when spawned in captivity compared with wild sh spawned under identical conditions, which is a clear demons trati on of adapta tion to captiv ity. We also documente d a tradeoff among the wild-born broodstock: Those with the greatest tness in a captive environment produc ed off spr ing that per- formed the worst in the wild. Speci cally, captive-born individuals with ve (the median) or more returning siblings (i.e., offspring of successful broodstock) avera ged 0.62 returning offsprin g in the wild, whereas captive-born individuals with less than ve siblings averag ed 2.05 retur ning offspri ng in the wild. These results dem- onstrate that a single generation in captivity can result in a sub- stanti al res pons e to selection on tra its tha t are benecial in captivity but severely maladaptive in the wild. sheries | genetics | parentage | rapid evolution | salmon C aptive breeding programs are commonly used for the con- servation of endangered species and, more recently, for the restoration of declining populations (14). Mounting evidence suggest s that captive-bor n indivi duals released into the wild can have substantially lower tness than their wild-born counterparts and that these tness declines can occur after only a few gen- erations in captivity (58). Identifying the mechanisms that cause reduced tness in the wild is vital for deciding if, when, and how captive breeding programs should be applied for conservation and management purp ose s (5, 7). Explanati ons for the rapid tness declines (812) include environmental effects of captive rearing (including heritable epigenetic effects), inbreeding among close relatives, relaxed natural selection, and unintentional do- mesti catio n sele ction (adaptati on to the novel environment). Each of these mechanisms creates subtle but testable differences in patterns of reproductive success. Environmental effects of captive rearing, for example, could produc e differ ences in tness between captive-b orn and wild- born individuals but would not create differences in tness among individuals that experienced identical captive environments (12, 13). Relaxed natural selection in captivity is a compelling hy- pothesis because it can manifest in a myriad of forms. Lack of mate choice, for example, could result in combinations of im- mune-related genes that do not maximize tness (14, 15). Nev- erthe less, theoreti cal analy ses suggest that for rela xed natura l selection to cause a rapid tness decline, the population must have a high standing mutational load or spend many generations in captiv ity (9). Uninte ntiona l domest icati on sele ction, on the other hand, can rapidly reduce tness in the wild, especially if multiple traits are under selection (10, 16). If unintentional domestication selection is occurring, we ex- pec t to observe two uniq ue pat ter ns. Fir st, captiv e-b orn indi-  viduals should perform better in captivity than wild-born individuals. Second, there should be a tradeoff among the wild- born broodstock: Those with the greatest tness in a captive environment will produce offspring that perform the worst in the  wild. These predictions are not expected under relaxed natural selection because individuals with t and unt genotypes (when expressed in the wild) would perform identically in a captive environment where that genetic variation is selectively neutral. We test these competing explanations with a detailed pedigree analysis of a wild steelhead (Oncorh ynchus mykiss ) population that was supple mented with captiv e-reared indivi duals. Bill ions of captiv e-rea red salmo n are intent ional ly released into the wild each year in efforts to increase shery yields, miti- gate environmental disturbances, and bolster severely declining populations (1719). Steelhead from the Hood River in Oregon are listed as threatened under the US Endangered Species Act (20), and part of their recovery plan includes supplementation  with juvenile sh produced in a captive breeding program (i.e., sh hatchery). For winter-run steelhead from this population, we constr ucted three-gene ration pedig rees from 15 run-ye ars by genotyping 12,700 sh at eight highly polymorphic microsatellite loci. Steelhead en route to their spawning grounds in the Hood River were rst passe d ove r the Powerdale Dam, whi ch was a complete barrier to migrating sh (SI Materials and Methods). Because of this barrier, we were able to obtain samples of every returning sh that spawned in the wild. Previous work from this system documented that captive-born sh with two wild parents averaged 85% of the repr oduc tiv e suc ces s of the ir wi ld-b orn counterparts (6). However, the mechanism responsible for the documented tness decline remained unknown. In this captiv e breed ing progra m, ocean-retur ning wild-born and rst-generation hatchery adults were collected from the wild and spa wne d in a hat che ry (he re aft er, broodstock ; Fi g. 1) . Their offspring (hereafter, F1sh) were reared in a hatchery envir onment and relea sed near wild-spawning habitat as juve- nile s. After releas e, the F1 sh went out to sea, returned as adults, and spawned in the wild. The progeny of F1 sh (here- after, F2sh) spent their entire lives in the wild. Using par- ent age ana lys es, we ass ign ed F1 hatche ry sh back to the ir broodstock parents. We again used parentage analysis to assign the returning, wild-born F2 sh back to their F1 parents. We Author contributions: M.R.C. and M.S.B. designed research; M.R.C., M.L.M., and R.A.F. performed research; M.R.C. analyzed data; and M.R.C. and M.S.B. wrote the paper. The authors declare no conict of interest. This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. F.W.A. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial Board. 1 To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]. edu. This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. 1073/pnas.1111073109/-/DCSupplemental . www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1111073109 PNAS Early Edition | 1 of 5       E       V       O       L       U       T       I       O       N

Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 1/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 2/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 3/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 4/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 5/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 6/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 7/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 8/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 9/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 10/11

7/31/2019 Christie Et Al.pnas2011[1]

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/christie-et-alpnas20111 11/11