18
ZOOLOGICAL SCIENCE 34: 267–274 (2017) © 2017 Zoological Society of Japan * Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected] doi:10.2108/zs160196 Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic Among Derived Groups Michio Hori 1 , Mifuyu Nakajima 2 , Hiroki Hata 3 , Masaki Yasugi 4 , Satoshi Takahashi 5 , Masanori Nakae 6 , Kosaku Yamaoka 7 , Masanori Kohda 8 , Jyun-ichi Kitamura 9 , Masayoshi Maehata 10 , Hirokazu Tanaka 8,11 , Norihiro Okada 12,13 , and Yuichi Takeuchi 14 * 1 Kyoto University, Yoshida-Honmachi, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan, 2 Department of Biology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA, 3 Graduate School of Science and Engineering, Ehime University, Bunkyocho 2-5, Matsuyama 790-8577, Japan, 4 National Institute for Basic Biology, Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi 444-8787, Japan, 5 Research Group of Environmental Sciences, Nara Woman’s University, Kitauoya Nishimachi, Nara 630-8506, Japan, 6 Department of Zoology, National Museum of Nature and Science, Amakubo 4-1-1, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-0005, Japan, 7 Kochi University, Monobe-B, Nankoku, Kochi 783-8502, Japan, 8 Department of Biological Science, Osaka City University, Sugimotocho, Sumiyoshi-ku, Osaka 558-8585, Japan, 9 Mie Prefectural Museum, Isshinden-kouzubeta 3060, Tsu, Mie 514-0061, Japan, 10 Kobe Gakuin University, Arise 518, Ikawadanicho, Nishi-ku, Kobe 651-2180, Japan, 11 Institute of Ecology and Evolu- tion, University of Bern, Hinterkappelen 3032, Switzerland, 12 Department of Life Sciences, National Cheng Kung University, Tainan 701, Taiwan, 13 Foundation for Advancement of International Science, Tsukuba 305- 0821, Japan, 14 Department of Anatomy, Graduate School of Medicine and Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of Toyama, Sugitani, Toyama 930-0194, Japan Laterality has been studied in several vertebrates, mainly in terms of brain lateralization and behav- ioral laterality, but morphological asymmetry has not been extensively investigated. Asymmetry in fishes was first described in scale-eating cichlids from Lake Tanganyika, in the form of bilateral dimorphism in which some individuals, when opening their mouths, twist them to the right and others to the left. This asymmetry has a genetic basis, and is correlated with lateralized attack behaviors. This has subsequently been found in fishes from numerous taxa with various feeding habits. The generality of such morphological laterality should thus be investigated in as wide a range of fishes as possible. Using specific indicators of lateral differences in mandibles and head inclination, we find that representative species from all 60 orders of extant gnathostome fishes (both bony and cartilaginous) possess morphological laterality. Furthermore, we identify the same laterality in agnathans (hagfish and lamprey), suggesting that this trait appeared early in fish evolu- tion and has been maintained across fish lineages. However, a comparison of asymmetry among groups of bony fishes reveals, unexpectedly, that phylogenetically more recent-groups possess less asymmetry in body structures. The universality of laterality in fishes indicates a monophyletic origin, and may have been present in the ancestors of vertebrates. Ecological factors, predator– prey interactions in particular, may be key drivers in the evolution and maintenance of dimorphism, and may also be responsible for the cryptic trend of asymmetry in derived groups. Because lung- fish and coelacanths share this trait, it is likely that tetrapods also inherited it. We believe that study of this morphological laterality will provide insights into the behavioral and sensory lateral- ization of vertebrates. Key words: morphological asymmetry, dimorphism, anti-symmetry, fish evolution, frequency-dependent selection INTRODUCTION Behavioral and sensory lateralization is widely recog- nized in various vertebrates (Roger and Andrew, 2002). Studies of lateralization have focused primarily on humans and higher vertebrates, mainly examining cerebral lateral- ization and related behavioral or sensory differentiation between right and left sides. However, morphological asym- metry, such as differences between the right and left sides of kinetic or sensory systems, has not been explored in depth, except for differences in the brain and nervous sys- tem. Recent studies of laterality in fishes have revealed that behavioral laterality is always associated with morphological asymmetry, which is characteristically represented as dimor- phism in populations, i.e., every population is composed of

Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

267Laterality is universal among fishesZOOLOGICAL SCIENCE 34: 267–274 (2017) © 2017 Zoological Society of Japan

* Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]:10.2108/zs160196

Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic Among Derived Groups

Michio Hori1, Mifuyu Nakajima2, Hiroki Hata3, Masaki Yasugi4, Satoshi Takahashi5, Masanori Nakae6, Kosaku Yamaoka7, Masanori Kohda8, Jyun-ichi Kitamura9,

Masayoshi Maehata10, Hirokazu Tanaka8,11, Norihiro Okada12,13, and Yuichi Takeuchi14*

1Kyoto University, Yoshida-Honmachi, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto 606-8501, Japan, 2Department of Biology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA, 3Graduate School of Science and Engineering, Ehime University,

Bunkyocho 2-5, Matsuyama 790-8577, Japan, 4National Institute for Basic Biology, Higashiyama 5-1, Myodaiji, Okazaki, Aichi 444-8787, Japan, 5Research Group of Environmental Sciences, Nara Woman’s University,

Kitauoya Nishimachi, Nara 630-8506, Japan, 6Department of Zoology, National Museum of Nature and Science, Amakubo 4-1-1, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305-0005, Japan, 7Kochi University, Monobe-B, Nankoku, Kochi 783-8502,

Japan, 8Department of Biological Science, Osaka City University, Sugimotocho, Sumiyoshi-ku, Osaka 558-8585, Japan, 9Mie Prefectural Museum, Isshinden-kouzubeta 3060, Tsu, Mie 514-0061, Japan, 10Kobe

Gakuin University, Arise 518, Ikawadanicho, Nishi-ku, Kobe 651-2180, Japan, 11Institute of Ecology and Evolu-tion, University of Bern, Hinterkappelen 3032, Switzerland, 12Department of Life Sciences, National Cheng Kung University, Tainan 701, Taiwan, 13Foundation for Advancement of International Science, Tsukuba 305-

0821, Japan, 14Department of Anatomy, Graduate School of Medicine and Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of Toyama, Sugitani, Toyama 930-0194, Japan

Laterality has been studied in several vertebrates, mainly in terms of brain lateralization and behav-ioral laterality, but morphological asymmetry has not been extensively investigated. Asymmetry in fishes was first described in scale-eating cichlids from Lake Tanganyika, in the form of bilateral dimorphism in which some individuals, when opening their mouths, twist them to the right and others to the left. This asymmetry has a genetic basis, and is correlated with lateralized attack behaviors. This has subsequently been found in fishes from numerous taxa with various feeding habits. The generality of such morphological laterality should thus be investigated in as wide a range of fishes as possible. Using specific indicators of lateral differences in mandibles and head inclination, we find that representative species from all 60 orders of extant gnathostome fishes (both bony and cartilaginous) possess morphological laterality. Furthermore, we identify the same laterality in agnathans (hagfish and lamprey), suggesting that this trait appeared early in fish evolu-tion and has been maintained across fish lineages. However, a comparison of asymmetry among groups of bony fishes reveals, unexpectedly, that phylogenetically more recent-groups possess less asymmetry in body structures. The universality of laterality in fishes indicates a monophyletic origin, and may have been present in the ancestors of vertebrates. Ecological factors, predator–prey interactions in particular, may be key drivers in the evolution and maintenance of dimorphism, and may also be responsible for the cryptic trend of asymmetry in derived groups. Because lung-fish and coelacanths share this trait, it is likely that tetrapods also inherited it. We believe that study of this morphological laterality will provide insights into the behavioral and sensory lateral-ization of vertebrates.

Key words: morphological asymmetry, dimorphism, anti-symmetry, fish evolution, frequency-dependent selection

INTRODUCTION

Behavioral and sensory lateralization is widely recog-nized in various vertebrates (Roger and Andrew, 2002). Studies of lateralization have focused primarily on humans and higher vertebrates, mainly examining cerebral lateral-

ization and related behavioral or sensory differentiation between right and left sides. However, morphological asym-metry, such as differences between the right and left sides of kinetic or sensory systems, has not been explored in depth, except for differences in the brain and nervous sys-tem. Recent studies of laterality in fishes have revealed that behavioral laterality is always associated with morphological asymmetry, which is characteristically represented as dimor-phism in populations, i.e., every population is composed of

Page 2: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

268 M. Hori et al.

both lefty and righty individuals at different rates. The goals of this paper are (1) to demonstrate that all fishes have later-ality and, therefore, that the origins of laterality are at least as old as those of fish, and (2) to discuss the significance of the maintenance of dimorphic laterality in most populations. Specifically, we direct our attention to predator–prey interac-tions in biotic communities as the primary factor responsible for the maintenance of dimorphism of laterality in fish.

The first and most notable example of laterally asym-metric bodies in fishes was found in scale-eating cichlids in Lake Tanganyika (Liem and Stewart, 1976; Hori, 1991, 1993; Hata et al., 2013), in which laterality was associated with hunting techniques. Each population consists of righty (the right side of the head faces slightly forward, and the mouth opens to the left) and lefty (vice versa) individuals. Righty (lefty) individuals attack the right (left) sides of their prey fish from behind to tear off scales (Hori, 1993; Takeuchi et al., 2012, 2016). The behavioral laterality associated with bilat-eral asymmetry has also been found in fishes from a wide range of trophic levels and taxa, including cichlids (Hori et al., 2007; Takeuchi and Hori, 2008), gobies (Seki et al., 2000; Yasugi and Hori, 2011), bettas (Takeuchi et al., 2010), large-mouth bass (Nakajima et al., 2007; Yasugi and Hori, 2011), characins (Hata et al., 2011), poeciliids (Matsui et al., 2013), and anglerfish (Yasugi and Hori, 2016). Nearly all of the indi-viduals in these studies could be classified as either righty or lefty individuals.

Laterality appears to have a genetic basis, as indicated by studies on parents and their offspring in wild species (a goby: Seki et al., 2000; two cichlids: Hori et al., 2007) and in breeding experiments (a cichlid: Hata et al., 2012; medaka: Hata et al., 2012; and zebrafish: Hata and Hori, 2012), in which righty and lefty offspring were born in a Mendelian ratio with left dominant over right. Additionally, genetic stud-ies have identified genomic loci corresponding to mandibu-lar asymmetry (Stewart and Albertson, 2010).

The degree of laterality in individuals has been quanti-fied based primarily on two measures: (i) differences between the right and left mandibles (Hori, 1993; Seki et al., 2000; Takeuchi and Hori, 2008; Yasugi and Hori, 2011; Takeuchi et al., 2012; Hata et al., 2013), more specifically, the height of the posterior ends of the right and left mandi-bles (HMPE) (Hori et al., 2007; Hata et al., 2013) (Fig. 1A), and (ii) the angle θ formed by the head and vertebrae (Hata et al., Yasugi and Hori, 2011; Nakajima et al., 2007) (Fig. 1B), in which the head of each individual inclines laterally, such that one side of the head faces forward (Hori et al., 2007). Laterality identified in every individual using these two meth-ods has been consistent (Yasugi and Hori, 2011; Hata et al., 2013).

In general, lateral asymmetry in species or populations can be divided into three categories based on the frequency distribution of measured laterality: fluctuating asymmetry (FA) with a unimodal and symmetrical distribution, direc-tional asymmetry (DA) with a unimodal distribution shifted from symmetry, and anti-symmetry (AS) with a bimodal distribution (Palmer and Strobeck, 1992). Recent studies examining the asymmetry of fishes have shown bimodal dis-tributions, suggesting AS (Hori et al., 2007; Nakajima et al., 2007; Takeuchi and Hori, 2008; Seki et al., 2000; Takeuchi et al., 2010, 2012, 2016; Hata et al., 2011, 2013; Yasugi and

Hori, 2011, 2012; Matsui et al., 2013). Here, to ascertain the type of asymmetry, we examined frequency distributions of two measures in two species of bony fishes—amberjack and catfish—as arbitrary representatives of dominant taxa in marine and freshwater environments, respectively, as well as in one cartilaginous fish, a stingray.

Next, to assess the prevalence of lateral asymmetry among fishes and to identify trends in the degree of laterality during evolution, we measured the laterality of individuals from at least one arbitrary representative species from all 62 extant orders of fishes (Nelson, 2006). The species exam-ined and the measurements of the 62 orders are listed in the electronic supplementary material (Supplementary Table S1). We measured mandibular asymmetry and head angle

Fig. 1. Bone measurements used to identify fish laterality. (A) The height of the mandible at the posterior end (HMPE) is the distance from A (the deepest point on the suspensoriad articulation facet of the anguloarticular) to RP (the ventral corner of the retroarticular process). (B) Ventral view of a skeleton showing data points for head angle (θ ), defined as the angle between the line extended from VIII–B (dashed red line) and P–B. B: the sagittal posterior end of the basioccipital; P: the center of the posterior portion of the parasphenoid; VIII: the sagittal posterior end of the third vertebra. Both panels show the Japanese amberjack (Seriola quinqueradiata).

Page 3: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

269Laterality is universal among fishes

(as described above) in 10–12 individuals from one to sev-eral populations of each species, except for five rare species in which only a few individuals (n < 6) were available.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Collection of fish specimensTo define the frequency distribution of laterality, we collected

approximately 100 individuals each of Japanese amberjack (Seri-ola quinqueradiata, Perciformes), Amur catfish (Silurus asotus, Siluriformes), and Japanese stingray (Dasyatis akajei) as arbitrary representatives of dominant marine and freshwater bony fish taxa, and a marine cartilaginous fish, respectively. Next, to determine the presence and degree of asymmetry in each fish order, we chose at least one arbitrary representative species from all 62 orders of extant fishes based on their availability. We then collected 10–12 individuals from one to several populations of each species, except for five rare species in which only a few individuals (n < 6) were available (see Supplementary Table S1 for details). Animal care and operations were performed in accordance with the Regulation on Animal Experimentation at Kyoto University.

Measurements of morphological laterality in fish specimensLaterality of gnathostome fishes was quantified by two mea-

sures: the difference in the height of the mandible at the posterior end (HMPE) of the right and left mandibles (Fig. 1A for bony fishes and electronic supplementary material, Supplementary Figure S2A for cartilaginous fish) and the angle from the vertebrae to the head (θ in Fig. 1B for bony fishes and Supplementary Figure S2B for cartilaginous fishes) following Hata et al. (2013). In cartilaginous fishes (except in chimaeras), the sustentaculum functions as the mandibular surface of the hyomandibular-mandibular joint, and the nodule on the ventral corner of the mandibular cartilage (VC) func-tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis (Dean and Motta, 2004; Wilga, 2005), such that the HMPE provides a functional measure equivalent to that of bony fishes. Despite its taxonomic position, the sturgeon is grouped with cartilaginous rather than bony fishes in this paper, as its skeleton is cartilaginous and the mechanical structure of its mouthparts resembles that of cartilaginous fishes (Carroll and Wainwright, 2003).

To prepare skeletal material, samples of all specimens, except those of the coelacanth, were gently boiled, and tissues were removed carefully by hand. For agnathan fishes (hagfish and lamprey), the mouthparts (a pair of spinose cartilage and flat carti-laginous bones, respectively, which may be homologous to the mandible of gnathostome fishes; Kuratani, 2012) were measured (Supplementary Figures S5A and S6A). We also measured the angle from the vertebrae to the head (Supplementary Figures S5B and S6B). Specimens of agnathan fishes were moderately boiled to expose mouthparts for measurement and then transparentized to measure θ. On each skeletal specimen of both gnathostome and agnathan fishes, the data points were marked with a fine pen under a binocular microscope, and HMPE and θ were measured using a digital microscope (VHX-100, Keyence Co., Inc., Osaka, Japan) to within 0.005 mm and 0.05° of accuracy, respectively. Data were measured horizontally using a level scope attached to a monocular microscope of our own design. Specifically, specimens were secured with a small clip that had a universal joint connected to a base comprising a magnet, which was then placed on a two-dimensional gonio-stage. Manipulating the gonio-stage and moni-toring via the level scope kept the specimens level. We used the median of three measurements from each specimen. Each mea-surement was obtained by repositioning the specimen on the microscope stage. For the coelacanth, computerized axial tomog-raphy (CAT) scan images of two frozen specimens stored at the Tokyo Institute of Technology were supplied, and the two measures were obtained using a DICOM viewer (INTAGE Realia, K.G.T., Inc.,

Tokyo, Japan). The index of asymmetry (IAS) for the difference between right and left mandible heights was given by [2 × (R − L) / (R + L)] × 100, where R and L are the right and left HMPE, respec-tively. Fish with positive IAS values and θ were defined as righty, and those with negative values were defined as lefty. Note that the definition of laterality used here and in recent studies differs from that used in earlier papers (Hori, 1991, 1993; Seki et al., 2000), which defined individuals with the mouth opening to the right as ‘right-handed’ or ‘dextral’. The terminology used in the present study, ‘lefty’, reflects the fact that the left mandible of such ‘right-handed’ fish is larger than the right mandible (Hori et al., 2007; Takeuchi and Hori, 2008; Yasugi and Hori, 2011, 2012), and that the left eye is dominant (Takeuchi et al., 2010).

Classification and statistical examination of asymmetryThe asymmetries of morphological characteristics were cate-

gorized as FA, DA, or AS for each species using the frequency dis-tributions of the laterality indices. To define the asymmetries of the IAS and θ in each species, we fitted three models to the frequency distributions of our measurements: (1) FA model, which assumes a normal distribution with a mean of 0 and standard deviation (s.d.) equal to the s.d. of the data; (2) DA model, which assumes a normal distribution of both the mean μ (≠ 0) and s.d. of the data; and (3) AS model, which assumes two normal distributions with the same s.d. and means at ±μ (bimodal distribution). The two normal distribu-tions with means +μ and −μ correspond to the frequency distribu-tions of righty and lefty fish, respectively. The means, s.d., and ratios of righty to lefty individuals in each model were estimated by maximum likelihood. For each species, we calculated the Akaike information criterion (AIC) of the three models using the R package IASD 1.0.7. The models with the lowest AIC values were selected as the best explanations of the asymmetries in morphological charac-teristics.

Phylogenetic grouping of fishesThe classification of extant fish orders used here follows

Nelson (2006). For evolutionary analysis, we categorized orders within Osteichthyes (bony fishes) as follows: (1) sarcopterygians (lobe-finned fishes), (2) primitive actinopterygians (ray-finned fishes), and (3) lower, (4) intermediate, and (5) higher teleosts (one of three infraclasses of ray-finned fishes). These categories were based on those proposed by Nelson (2006), but are slightly modi-fied in light of recent phylogenetic hypotheses, as shown in Supple-mentary Figure S1 (Betancur-R et al., 2013). Although Nelson et al. (2016) changed their classification of extant orders from those in Nelson (2006), the fish categories mentioned above are consistent with those of the former (group memberships are shown in Fig. 3A and Supplementary Table S1). Within the teleosts, we also catego-rized fishes into an ecological group ‘deep-sea fishes’ (those living at depths below 200 m) due to their distinctive degrees of asym-metry (see Discussion). The degree of laterality of each category was calculated as the average distance for laterality intensity (DLI), which was measured from the origin to a point (x, y) representing each species, where x is the standardized absolute value of IAS and y is the standardized absolute value of θ.

Degree of lateral asymmetry during growthTo compare the degree of lateral asymmetry among fishes of

various ages, we collected 60 largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides) individuals of various lengths from a single population in Lake Biwa, Japan, in 2004–2008. We measured the standard length, fish age by counting circuli on the opercular bone (Menon, 1950; Campana, 2001), IAS, and θ of every individual. Then we used analysis of variance (ANOVA) to assess the effects of age on IAS and θ.

Page 4: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

270 M. Hori et al.

Fig. 2. Frequency dis-tributions of (A) the index of asymmetry (IAS) of the mandibles and (B) head angle (θ ) of the Japanese amber-jack (Seriola quinquera-diata, n = 100; upper) and Amur catfish (Silurus asotus, n = 110; lower). Lines show the proba-bility densities derived from the three models (see Methods). The model selected by the lowest Akaike informa-tion criterion (AIC) value is indicated by the thick line.

Fig. 3. Degrees of laterality among extant teleost fishes. (A) Laterality quantified by the abso-lute values of the index of asym-metry (IAS) of the mandibles and the head angle (θ ) in each repre-sentative species from all extant orders of teleost fishes. Index numbers correspond to the extant orders of fishes in Nelson (2006). Species are grouped into six cat-egories (deep-sea fishes and five phylogenetic groups) indicated by colors. Each axis was standard-ized by the mean and standard deviation (s.d.) of all original val-ues. (B) Comparison of the dis-tance of laterality intensity (DLI) between the seven species of deep-sea fishes and the other teleost fishes (range, outlier, lower and upper quartiles, and median). P-values are from the Wilcoxon rank sum test. **, P < 0.01. (C) Comparison of DLI among five phylogenetic groups (range, outlier, lower and upper quartiles, and median).

Page 5: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

271Laterality is universal among fishes

RESULTS

Types of asymmetryOur measurements of amberjack and catfish were best

explained by the AS model, which indicates that the focal laterality in both species was AS. Histograms of mandibular asymmetry and head angle showed bimodal distributions in both sexes, with few individuals near 0 (Fig. 2). AS was the best-fitting model for these distributions among the three asymmetry types, per the AIC values. AS has been detected in the laterality of several fish orders (Takeuchi and Hori, 2008; Yasugi and Hori, 2011; Hata et al., 2011, 2013; Matsui et al., 2013) and was also found in a cartilaginous fish, the Japanese stingray, in the present study (Supplementary Fig-ure S2).

Except for the six species with small sample sizes, every species examined was composed of both righty and lefty individuals, and the absolute degree of laterality was always greater than 0 for both measures (Supplementary Table S1). The degree of laterality measured in the present study did not appear to change during growth (Supplementary Figure S3). The distributions of both measures were mostly explained by the AS model, with only three exceptions in mandibular asymmetry (Supplementary Table S1). Among the exceptions, two showed DA, which is likely due to biased proportions of morphs among the samples (one or two indi-viduals of minority morph). The third exception was a flatfish (Pleuronectiformes), which showed FA. However, our mea-surements of head angle in flatfishes demonstrated that each population was composed of both righty and lefty indi-viduals. Flatfishes have DA bodies with both eyes on the same side of the face. The upper and lower jaws on the side with the eyes are usually shorter than those on the opposite side. The IAS of the mandibles in flatfishes may be biased by the restricted form of the mouthparts (Supplementary Figure S4). In the likelihood ratio test between FA and AS, AS was significantly better than FA for both measures among all species with ≥10 samples (Supplementary Table S1). Therefore, all fishes may exhibit laterality.

Although agnathans have no mandibles, their mouthparts and head angle showed AS distributions (Supplementary Figures S5 and S6). Therefore, the phylogenetic occurrence of focal laterality may date back to the origin of fishes.

Trends in fish phylogenyThe absolute values of our two laterality measures (IAS

of the mandibles and θ ) in all orders of extant bony fishes revealed functional and evolutionary trends (Fig. 3A). First, deep-sea fishes had high levels of asymmetry except for one species (jellynose fish). The distances from the origin of the two axes in Fig. 3A to the points representing each spe-cies (DLI) were significantly greater for the seven deep-sea fishes than for the other fishes (Wilcoxon rank sum test, n = 45, P = 0.010; Fig. 3B). Because the exploitation of extreme depths evolved independently among deep-sea fishes (Nelson, 2006), extreme asymmetry seems adaptive for life in the deep sea, as discussed below. Second, the absolute values of IAS and θ in other fishes had a significant negative correlation (Spearman’s rank correlation test, n = 38, ρ = − 0.402, P = 0.012; Fig. 3A), suggesting trade-offs between the two structures in developing laterality.

‘Ancient fishes,’ such as lungfish, coelacanths, and gars, had greater DLIs than ‘modern fishes,’ such as perciforms and scorpaeniforms. We categorized bony fishes (except the deep-sea fishes) into five phylogenetic groups (see Meth-ods). The DLI values had a significant negative correlation with the phylogenetic order of the groups (Spearman’s rank correlation test, n = 38, ρ= − 0.352, P = 0.030; Fig. 3C), implying that the degree of morphological laterality was reduced progressively during the evolution of bony fishes. The phylogenetic trends in laterality among cartilaginous fishes are shown in Supplementary Figure S7; more derived species, such as rays and skates, had lower DLI values.

DISCUSSION

Our results indicate that AS laterality is universally shared among fishes, but that the degree of asymmetry has become increasingly weaker in more derived groups. How-ever, functional (behavioral) laterality seems not to be reduced even in derived groups, since many fishes have prominent behavioral laterality, including cichlids (Hori, 1993; Takeuchi and Hori, 2008), gobies (Seki et al., 2000), largemouth bass (Nakajima et al., 2007; Yasugi and Hori, 2011), and bettas (Takeuchi et al., 2010), all of which belong to derived groups. This cryptic trend in morphological later-ality should be considered in depth. Here, we discuss 1) why morphological and behavioral laterality has developed in fish and why fish have AS rather than DA laterality; 2) the relationship between the two measures we used to evaluate morphological laterality, its implications for body form, and how this relates to the progressive weakening of morpho-logical laterality in fish evolution; 3) the origin and legacy of fish laterality; and 4) the relationship between laterality in fishes and that in aquatic invertebrates.

Behavioral advantage has been proposed as a driving force behind lateral asymmetry among fishes. The general fish body plan facilitates a bilateral motor pattern. The body is propelled by side-to-side undulations. When a fish makes a dashing movement, it first bends its body either to the right or left and subsequently straightens it (Wilga, 2005). The locomotive speed of this initial motion is crucial in predator–prey interactions (Cantalupo et al., 1995; Takeuchi et al., 2012; Yasugi and Hori, 2012). If one side of the body is mechanically dominant, i.e., has more muscle and effective support tissue than the opposite side, the initial use of this side can produce stronger propulsion and higher speed than that generated by non-specialists with completely symmetri-cal bodies (Takeuchi et al., 2012). Therefore, morphological asymmetry and laterally disproportionate usage (behavioral laterality) may be associated and mutually reinforcing.

Although asymmetrical bodies seem advantageous, why are fish dimorphic rather than monomorphic? Behav-ioral laterality inevitably leads to biased directions of attack (Nakajima et al., 2007; Yasugi and Hori, 2012) and evasion (Izvekov et al., 2009; Yasugi and Hori, 2012). These biases may provoke negative, frequency-dependent selection. For instance, of the two lateral morphs among scale-eaters, the numerically dominant morph was more likely to fail at feed-ing than the less common morph, probably because prey fishes are more vigilant on the side more frequently attacked by the major morph (Hori, 1993; Takahashi and Hori, 1994, 1998). Among pursuit-type piscivores, predation success

Page 6: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

272 M. Hori et al.

was higher if the laterality of the predator and its prey were opposite (so-called ‘cross predation’) than if they were the same (‘parallel predation’) (Yasugi and Hori, 2011, 2012). Conversely, when luring-type piscivores face prey fish dur-ing a predation event, parallel predation was more success-ful (Yasugi and Hori, 2016). This difference in success also seems to cause an alternating advantage between righties and lefties in both predator and prey fishes and results in the dynamic maintenance of laterality (Nakajima et al., 2004). This advantage is shared by predators that attack the same prey fishes and by prey fishes that share a common predator (Takahashi and Hori, 2005). Natural selection through pred-ator-prey interactions may play a major role in maintaining anti-symmetric dimorphism in fish communities (Stearns and Hoekstra, 2000; Hedrick, 2005; Sinervo and Calsbeek, 2006). We suggest that this is the very reason why morpho-logical laterality has progressively weakened during fish evolution, which we discuss below.

The two measures of fish laterality used here represent the most distinctly asymmetric features. The morphological implications of head angle are intuitive. If the body is asym-metric due to differential development of the right and left sides, the more developed side may be structurally domi-nant over the opposite side. The more developed side should, therefore, be convex, as suggested in Fig. 1. Zebrafish were found to have a trunk muscle volume that was laterally biased in relation to the laterality of the fish (Heuts, 1999), although there was a significant bias towards escaping in one direction at the population level. The mor-phological implication of the IAS of the HMPE can be also understood by applying the principle of leverage; i.e., the HMPE acts as a line between the effort point (i.e., where the ligament is attached) and the fulcrum point of the lever (i.e., the articulation of the mandible) (Westneat, 1990). Thus, the difference between the right and left HMPE may cause the force and speed of opening to differ between the right and left mandibles, causing the mouth to twist as it opens. Because of this morphological asymmetry, the fish can open its mouth toward one side to a greater extent than through head inclination alone. In any lateralized behavior, these fac-tors may work together. For example, in the shrimp-eating cichlid (Neolamprologus fasciatus; Takeuchi and Hori, 2008) and the scale-eating cichlid (Perissodus microlepis; Takeuchi et al., 2016), each morph exhibits an advantage (greater hunting success) when the fish uses the dominant side of its body during foraging. The direction to which the body was bent, the eye used to aim at the target, and the dominant side of the mandible were all consistent.

The negative relationship between the two measures found among extant bony fishes (Fig. 3A) suggests a trade-off between asymmetry in the mandibles and head angle. Although this relationship may reflect the constraints imposed by morphogenesis, it may be ultimately affected by differences in lifestyles and modes of locomotion among fish groups. Fishes with a large head angle, such as silvereye, John Dory, and puffer fish (#42, 54, and 60, respectively, in Fig. 3A), are deep-bodied, whereas those with high IAS val-ues, such as herring, salmon, and pickerel (#27, 35, and 36, respectively, in Fig. 3A), have slender bodies (defined as having a high body length to diameter ratio). Perciforms are highly variable in body shape. Among them, slender fishes,

such as amberjack, are adapted to prolonged high-speed swimming and a pelagic lifestyle, whereas deep-bodied fishes, such as sea bream, are adapted to quick turns and a demersal lifestyle (Azuma, 2006). This relationship between body shape and swimming style may apply to all bony fishes except those with extreme body shapes, such as deep-sea fishes and eels. Thus, we propose that deep-bodied fishes are suited to tilting the head to either side during asymmetric behaviors, whereas slender fishes are suited to having asymmetric mandibles due to their streamlined bodies and smaller head angles adapted to rapid and prolonged cruis-ing. A more symmetrical bodyline is adaptive for high-speed swimming (Gosline, 1971). One of the fastest-moving fishes, the flying fish Prognichthys brevipinnis, has the smallest head angle (Supplementary Table S1).

The universality of morphological laterality in fishes sug-gests a monophyletic origin. We hypothesize that the evolu-tion of fish laterality began with lancelets or agnathans, and that laterality has been maintained over the course of fish evolution. Ecological factors, particularly predator–prey interactions, are likely key drivers in the evolution of lateral dimorphism in every fish population, which should be main-tained by frequency-dependent selection mediated through minority advantage (Hori, 1993; Stearns and Hoekstra, 2000; Hedrick, 2005; Sinervo and Calsbeek, 2006). Although laterality is inevitable for effective locomotion in every fish species, they must also conceal their own laterality from appearance, as such laterality would indicate a preferred attacking direction of predators or escape direction of prey. Laterality may therefore have become increasingly cryptic during fish evolution. In this context, the extreme asymmetry in deep-sea fishes (Fig. 3A) may be explained as follows; since they live in the dark and rely only slightly on vision, they have little need to conceal their morphological laterality.

In addition to the origin of laterality, the remarkable later-ality of sarcopterygians (coelacanths and lungfish; #61 and 62, respectively, in Fig. 3A), which share a common ances-tor with terrestrial tetrapods (amphibians, reptiles, birds, and mammals), provides another implication for the universality of laterality, particularly with respect to the traits inherited by tetrapods from fish. Lateralized behaviors have been evalu-ated in animals ranging from fish to mammals (Roger and Andrew, 2002) with emphasis on brain lateralization, which is thought to facilitate enhanced performance or faster responses (Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005; Dadda et al., 2010). By contrast, the laterality of behaviors in fishes, including hunting techniques (Hori, 1993; Nakajima et al., 2007; Takeuchi and Hori, 2008; Hata et al., 2011; Yasugi and Hori, 2012; Takeuchi et al., 2012, 2016), scouting behaviors (Matsui et al., 2013), and social behaviors (Takahashi and Hori, 2008; Takeuchi et al., 2010), are correlated with the morphological laterality that we have demonstrated here. This documentation of morphological laterality in fish may lead to a deeper understanding of laterality in other verte-brates.

Morphological AS has also been known in aquatic inver-tebrate groups, including Arthropoda and Mollusca (Palmer, 2005). Our recent studies on shrimp (Takeuchi et al., 2008), crayfish (Tobo et al., 2012), and cuttlefish (Lucky et al., 2012) demonstrated that their morphological AS is associated with behavioral laterality, which is involved with escaping from

Page 7: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

273Laterality is universal among fishes

predators or attacking prey. Like fish, these animals move actively in aquatic environments. They suffer minimal effects from gravity due to buoyancy, which may allow them to develop bilateral body asymmetry. By contrast, in terrestrial environments, the effect of gravity may force animals to have a laterally symmetric body supported by limbs in a bal-anced arrangement. Morphological laterality in terrestrial animals may thus have been weakened, although functional (behavioral) laterality seems to remain, as many studies indicate (e.g., Rogers and Andrew, 2002). Although it is dif-ficult to speculate on the phylogenetic relationship of lateral-ity between aquatic invertebrates and fishes, it is possible that all members of Bilateria share laterality as a homology, given the commonness of morphological laterality among invertebrate phyla (Palmer, 2005). In addition, Arthropoda and Mollusca are phylogenetically older than fishes (Moody and Zhuravlev, 2001), and behavioral laterality seems to have already existed among Cambrian trilobites and/or their predators (Babcock, 1993); fish did not appear until the end of this period. Therefore, it is possible that laterality devel-oped first in Arthropoda, and then probably in Cephalopoda (e.g., Nautiloidea in the Ordovician: Babcock, 1993). The Cambrian also saw the explosive development of predator–prey interactions, which were driven by newly acquired eye-sight in most animal phyla (Gould, 1989). We speculate that the development of the anti-symmetric laterality in Bilateria may be closely linked to such interspecific interactions where appearance is crucial to detect prey and/or escape from predators. Although it is unclear whether the laterality of fish is homologous with that of aquatic invertebrates, the anti-symmetric laterality of fish may have been developed through predator-prey interactions in ancient aquatic com-munities. Understanding the laterality of fish may also shed new light on the community structures of aquatic ecosys-tems.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank all the institutes, museums, aquariums, researchers, and fishermen who provided the fish materials listed in Supplemen-tary Table S1, and Drs. M. Aibara, R. Takahashi, and N. Ogihara for the CAT scan image data analysis of the coelacanth. This work was funded by grants from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, Japan (21st Century COE Program (A14), Global COE Program (A06), Priority Areas (14087203), and KAKENHI (21370010, 15H05230, 16H05773, 17K14934)).

COMPETING INTERESTS

The authors have no competing interests to declare.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

MH planned and headed the project, and wrote the manuscript. MNakajima collected the materials and refined the manuscript. MH, HH, MY, MNakae, KY, MK, JK, MM, HT, and YT collected materials and carried out the measurements, ST and HT cover the statistical examination, and NO obtained coelacanth specimens and their CT image data. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS

Supplementary materials for this article are available online (URL: http://www.bioone.org/doi/suppl/10.2108/zs160196).

Supplementary Table S1. List of species analyzed and

model-fitting results.Supplementary Figure S1. Classification and phylogenetic

tree of the extant orders of teleost fishes used in this study.Supplementary Figure S2. Laterality of the Japanese sting-

ray (Dasyatis akajei).Supplementary Figure S3. Relationship between growth and

laterality in largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides).Supplementary Figure S4. Laterality of the flatfishes.Supplementary Figure S5. Laterality of the hagfish.Supplementary Figure S6. Laterality of the lamprey.Supplementary Figure S7. Degrees of laterality in extant car-

tilaginous fish orders and sturgeons.

REFERENCES

Azuma A (2006) The biokinetics of flying and swimming 2nd ed, American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics, Virginia

Babcock LE (1993) Trilobite malformations and the fossil record of behavioral asymmetry. J Paleontol 67: 217–229

Betancur-RR, Broughton RE, Wiley EO, Carpenter K, López JA, Li C, et al. (2013) The Tree of Life and a New Classification of Bony Fishes. PLOS Currents Tree of Life

Campana S (2001) Accuracy, precision and quality control in age determination, including a review of the use and abuse of age validation methods. J Fish Biol 59: 197–242

Cantalupo C, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (1995) Lateralization of predator-evasion response in a teleost fish (Girardinus falcatus). Neuropsychologia 33: 1637–1646

Carroll AM, Wainwright PC (2003) Functional morphology of prey capture in the sturgeon, Scaphirhynchus albus. J Morphol 256: 270–284

Dadda M, Koolhaas WH, Domenici P (2010) Behavioural asymme-try affects escape performance in a teleost fish. Biol Lett 6: 414–417

Dean N, Motta PJ (2004) Anatomy and functional morphology of the feeding apparatus of the lesser electric ray, Narcine brasiliensis(Elasmobranchii: Batoidea). J Morphol 262: 462–483

Gosline WA (1971) Functional Morphology and Classification of Teleostean Fishes. The university press of Hawaii, Hawaii

Gould SJ (1989) Wonderful life; the Burgess shale and the nature of history. WW Norton, New York

Hata H, Hori M (2012) Inheritance patterns of morphological lateral-ity in mouth opening of zebrafish, Danio rerio. Laterality 17: 741–754

Hata H, Yasugi M, Hori M (2011) Jaw laterality and related handed-ness in the hunting behavior of a scale-eating characin, Exodon paradoxus. PLoS ONE 6: e29349

Hata H. Takahashi R, Ashiwa H, Awata S, Takeyama T, Kohda M, Hori M (2012) Inheritance patterns of lateral dimorphism exam-ined by breeding experiments with the Tanganyikan cichlid (Julidochromis transcriptus) and the Japanese medaka (Oryzias latipes). Zool Sci 29: 49–53

Hata H, Yasugi M, Takeuchi Y, Hori M (2013) Distinct lateral dimor-phism in the jaw morphology of the scale-eating cichlids, Perrisodus microlepis and P. straeleni. Ecol Evol 3: 4641–4647

Hedrick PW (2005) Genetics of populations 3rd ed, Jones and Bartlett Publishers, Boston

Heuts B (1999) Lateralization of trunk muscle volume, and lateral-ization of swimming turns of fish responding to external stimuli. Behav Process 47: 113–124

Hori M (1991) Feeding relationships among cichlid fishes in Lake Tanganyika: effects of intra- and interspecific variation of feed-ing behavior on their coexistence. Ecol Int Bull 19: 89–101

Hori M (1993) Frequency-dependent natural selection in the hand-edness of scale-eating cichlid fish. Science 260: 216–219

Hori M, Ochi H, Kohda M (2007) Inheritance pattern of lateral dimor-phism in two cichlids (a scale eater, Perissodus microlepis, and

Page 8: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

274 M. Hori et al.

an herbivore, Neolamprologus moorii) in Lake Tanganyika. Zool Sci 24: 489–492

Inoue JG, Miya M (2001) Phylogeny of the basal teleosts, with spe-cial reference to the Elopomorpha. Japan J Ichthyol 48: 75–91

Izvekov EI, Chebotareva Yu V, Izyumov Yu G, Nepomnyashechikh VA, Medyantseva EN (2009) Behavioral and morphological asymmetries in roach Rutilus rutilus (Cyprinidae: Cypriniformes) underyearlings. J Ichthyol 49: 88–95

Kuratani S (2012) Evolution of the vertebrate jaw: comparative embryology and molecular developmental biology reveal the factors behind evolutionary novelty. J Anat 205: 335–347

Liem KF, Stewart DJ (1976) Evolution of the scale-eating cichlid fishes of Lake Tanganyika: a generic revision with a description of a new species. Bull Mus Compar Zool 147: 319–350

Lucky NS, Ihara R, Yamaoka K, Hori M (2012) Behavioral laterality and morphological asymmetry in the cuttlehish, Sepia lycidas. Zool Sci 29: 286–292

Matsui S, Takeuchi Y, Hori M (2013) Relation between morphologi-cal antisymmetry and behavioral laterality in a Poeciliid fish. Zool Sci 30: 613–618

Menon MO (1950) The use of bones other than otoliths in determin-ing the age and growth rate of fishes. J Cons Int Explor Mer 16: 311–335

Moody RTJ, Zhuravlev AY (2001) Atlas of the evolving earth. Andromeda Oxford Ltd, Oxfordshire

Nakajima M, Matsuda H, Hori M (2004) Persistence and fluctuation of lateral dimorphism in fishes. Am Nat 163: 692–698

Nakajima M, Yodo T, Katano O (2007) Righty fish are hooked on the right side of their mouths – observations from an angling exper-iment with largemouth bass, Michroperus salmoides. Zool Sci 24: 855–859

Near TJ, Dornburg A, Eytan RI, Keck BP, Smith WL, Kuhn KL, et al. (2013) Phylogeny and tempo of diversification in the superra-diation of spiny-rayed fishes. PNAS Early Edition: 1–6

Nelson JS (2006) Fishes of the World 4th ed, Wiley, New JerseyNelson JS, Grande TC, Wilson MVH (2016) Fishes of the World 5th

ed, Wiley, New JerseyPalmer AR, Strobeck C (1992) Fluctuating asymmetry as a mea-

sure of development stability: implications of non-normal distri-butions and power of statistical tests. Acta Zool Fennica 191: 57–72

Roger LJ, Andrew RJ (2002) Comparative Vertebrate Lateralization. Cambridge University Press, London

Seki S, Kohda M, Hori M (2000) Asymmetry of mouth morph of a freshwater goby, Rhinogobius flumineus. Zool Sci 17: 1321–1325

Sinervo, B, Calsbeek R (2006) The developmental, physiological, neural, and genetical causes and consequences of frequency-dependent selection in the wild. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst 37: 581–610

Stearns SC, Hoekstra RF (2000) Evolution: An Introduction. Oxford University Press, Oxford

Stewart TA, Albertson RC (2010) Evolution of a unique predatory

feeding apparatus: functional anatomy, development and a genetic locus for jaw laterality in Lake Tanganyika scale-eating cichlids. BMC Biology 8: 8

Takahashi S, Hori M (1994) Unstable evolutionarily stable strategy and oscillation: a model on lateral asymmetry in scale-eating cichlids. Am Nat 144: 1001–1020

Takahashi S, Hori M (1998) Oscillation maintains polymorphisms - a model of lateral asymmetry in two competing scale-eating cichlids. J Theor Biol 195: 1–12

Takahashi S, Hori M (2005) Coexistence of competing species by the oscillation polymorphism. J Theor Biol 235: 591–596

Takahashi T, Hori M (2008) Evidence of disassortative mating in a Tanganyikan cichlid fish and its role in the maintenance of intra-population dimorphism. Biol Lett 4: 497–499

Takeuchi Y, Hori M (2008) Behavioural laterality in the shrimp-eating cichlid fish, Neolamprologus fasciatus, in Lake Tanganyika. Anim Behav 75: 1359–1366

Takeuchi Y, Tobo S, Hori M (2008) Morphological asymmetry of the abdomen and behavioral laterality in atyid shrimps. Zool Sci 25: 355–363

Takeuchi Y, Hori M, Myint O, Kohda M (2010) Lateral bias of agonis-tic responses to mirror images and morphological asymmetry in the Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). Behav Brain Res 208: 106–111

Takeuchi Y, Hori M, Oda Y (2012) Lateralized kinematics of preda-tion behavior in a Lake Tanganyika scale-eating cichlid fish. PLoS ONE 7: e29272

Takeuchi Y, Hori M, Tada S, Oda Y (2016) Acquisition of lateralized predation behavior associated with development of mouth asymmetry in a Lake Tanganyika scale-eating cichlid fish. PLoS ONE 11: e0147476

Tobo S, Takeuchi Y, Hori M (2012) Morphological asymmetry and behavioral laterality in the crayfish, Procambarus clarkii. Ecol Res 27: 53–59

Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ (2005) Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behav Brain Sci 28: 575–633

Westneat M (1990) Feeding mechanics of teleost fishes (Labridae, Perciformes) - a test of 4-bar linkage models. J Morphol 205: 269–295

Wilga CD (2005) Morphology and evolution of the jaw suspension in Lamniform sharks. J Morphol 265: 102–119

Yasugi M, Hori M (2011) Predominance of cross-predation between lateral morphs in a largemouth bass and a freshwater goby. Zool Sci 28: 869–874

Yasugi M, Hori M (2012) Lateralized behavior in the attacks of large-mouth bass on Rhinogobius gobies corresponding to their morphological antisymmetry. J Exper Biol 215: 2390–2398

Yasugi M, Hori M (2016) Predominance of parallel- and cross-pre-dation in anglerfish. Mar Ecol 37: 576–587

(Received November 16, 2016 / Accepted April 6, 2017)

Page 9: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Corrigendum: Laterality is universal among fishes but increasingly cryptic among derived groups

Michio Hori, Mifuyu Nakajima, Hiroki Hata, Masaki Yasugi, Satoshi Takahashi, Masanori Nakae, Kosaku Yamaoka, Masanori Kohda, Jyun-ichi Kitamura, Masayoshi Maehata, Hirokazu Tanaka, Norihiro Okada, and Yuichi Takeuchi

There was an error in Figure 2 in the published article. The model selected by the lowest Akaike information criterion value is indicated by the thick line (AS model).

Fig. 2. Frequency distributions of (A) the index of asymmetry (IAS) of the mandibles and (B) head angle (θ) of the Japanese amberjack (Seriola quinqueradiata, n = 100; upper) and Amur catfish (Silurus asotus, n = 110; lower). Lines show the probability densities derived from the three models (see Methods). The model selected by the lowest Akaike information criterion (AIC) value is indicated by the thick line.

Page 10: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Supplementary information

Laterality is universal among fishes but increasingly cryptic among derived groups

Supplementary Table 1. List of the species analyzed and the model-fitting results. Fish species analyzed as represen-tatives of each extant fish orders are listed with both Latin and common names, sample sizes, Akaike information crite-rion (AIC) values of the model fitted to the two measure-ments (bold: AIC of the selected model), sampling locations, and sources. P-values in LRT are from likelihood ratio test between FA model and AS model. Common names follow those used by FishBase (http://www.fishbase.org/). The index numbers of fish orders follow the classification used in Nelson (2006). Phylogenetic groupings for teleosts in this study, however, are slightly modified from Nelson’s system based on Betancur-R et al. (2013).Supplementary Figure 1. Classification and phylogenetic tree of the extant orders of teleost fishes used in this study. Phylogenetic trees are modified following Betancur-R et al. (2013). The classification is based on Nelson (2006), with a slight modification following Betancur-R et al. (2013) as shown in Supplementary Table 1 (colors are the same as those in Fig. 3A). The blue plot shows deep-sea fishes.Supplementary Figure 2. Laterality of the Japanese stingray (Dasyatis akajei). (A, B) Cartilaginous bones showing the data points used for measurements. (A) Ventral view of the jaw arch and a close-up of the left mandibular cartilage showing the data points for the index of asymmetry (IAS) of the mandibles. The arrow indicates the height of the mandible at the posterior end (HMPE), which is the distance from the postero-dorsal edge of the sustentaculum in the mandibular cartilage (PE) to the ventral corner of the man-dibular cartilage (VC). In cartilaginous fishes (except in chi-maeras), the sustentaculum functions as the mandibular surface of the hyomandibular-mandibular joint, and the nod-ule on the VC functions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis (Dean and Motta, 2004; Wilga 2005), such that the HMPE provides a functional measure equivalent to that of teleost fishes. (B) Ventral view of head and vertebrae showing data points for the head angle (θ ), which is the angle between the head and vertebrae on the ventral side of the anterior part of the skeleton. CC: the small foramen near the center of the cranial cartilage; PC: the mid-line of the postero-ventral margin of the cranium; PV: the first large process on the ventral sagittal line of the vertebra. (C, D) Frequency distributions of the (C) IAS of the mandibles and (D) θ in the Japanese stingray. Lines show the probabil-ity densities derived from the three models (see Methods). The model selected by the lowest Akaike information crite-rion (AIC) value is indicated by the thick line.Supplementary Figure 3. Relationship between growth and laterality in largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoi-des). The absolute values of the index of asymmetry (IAS) of mandibles and the head angles (θ ) in three size classes did not differ significantly (ANOVA, P = 0.2379 for IAS and P = 0.7791 for θ ). Individuals were sampled from the same pop-ulation in Lake Biwa, Japan, in 2004–2008. Vertical bars indicate standard deviations (s.d.). Small fish were primarily 0 + (n = 20, standard length [SL; mean ± s.d.] = 76.1 ± 4.1

mm), medium fish were primarily 1+ (n = 20, SL = 125.1 ± 13.6 mm), and large fish were primarily ≥ 2 + (n = 20, SL = 173.4 ± 16.8 mm).Supplementary Figure 4. Laterality of the flatfishes. Fre-quency distributions of the (A) index of symmetry (IAS) of the mandibles and (B) head angles in flathead flounder (Hip-poglossoides dubius; upper) and olive flounder (Paralich-thys olivaceus; lower). The lines are the same as those used in Figure 2. In both species, the model with the best fit to the distributions of IAS was fluctuating asymmetry, whereas the best model for head angle was anti-symmetry (see Supple-mentary Table 1).Supplementary Figure 5. Laterality of the hagfish. (A, B) Data for measurements from inshore hagfish (Eptatretus burgeri). (A) Ventral view of the mouthparts of a moderately boiled fish sample. A pair of flat cartilaginous structures behind the tooth plates is exposed. FC: the length of the flat cartilaginous structure used for the index of asymmetry (IAS). (B) Dorsal view of the anterior part of the body in a transparent preparation showing data for the head angle. CN: the center of the cartilaginous rings of the nasal tube; MC: the posterior end of the membranous cranium; DP: the dorsal datum point equidistant from MC to CN–MC on the sagittal line. Head angle (θ ) is defined as the angle of the line extending anteriorly from MC–DP to CN–MC. A fish with a clockwise positive angle is defined as left type, whereas a fish with a negative angle is defined as right type. The defini-tions of right and left types are consistent with those shown in the ventral view of Figure 1. (C, D) Frequency distributions of (C) IAS of the flat cartilaginous structure and (D) θ in inshore hagfish. The lines are the same as those used in Figure 2. Both distributions showed anti-symmetry (see Supplementary Table 1).Supplementary Figure 6. Laterality of the lamprey. (A, B) Data points used for measurements in arctic lamprey (Lampetra japonica). (A) Ventral view of a mouthpart in which a pair of spinose cartilaginous structures behind the suction cup are exposed in a moderately boiled fish sample. SC: the length of the spinose cartilage used for the index of asymmetry (IAS). (B) Dorsal view of the anterior part of the body in a transparent specimen showing the data points used for head angle (θ ) on the ventral sagittal line. PD: the anterior end of the posterior dorsal cartilage; PO: the poste-rior end of the postero-occipital cartilaginous ring; DP: the dorsal datum point equidistant to PD–PO. θ is defined as the angle from the line extending anteriorly from PO–DP to PD–PO. A fish with a clockwise positive angle is defined as left type, whereas a fish with a negative angle is defined as right type. (C, D) Frequency distributions of the (C) IAS of spinose cartilaginous structures and (D) θ in arctic lamprey. The lines are the same as those in Figure 2. Both distributions showed anti-symmetry (see Supplementary Table 1).Supplementary Figure 7. The degree of laterality in extant cartilaginous fish orders and sturgeons. Two measures of the laterality, the index of asymmetry (IAS) of the mandibles and the head angle, are displayed as in Fig-ure 3A. Fish species are grouped into four color-coded cat-egories.

Supplementary References

1. Betancur-RR, Broughton RE, Wiley EO, Carpenter K, Lopez JA,

Page 11: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Li C, Holcroft NI, Arcila D, Sanciangco M, Cureton J, Zhang F, Buser T, Campbell M, Rowley T, Ballesteros JA, Lu G, Grande T, Arratia G, Ortí G (2013) The tree of life and a new classifica-tion of bony fishes. PLoS Currents Tree of Life.

2. Nelson JS (2006) Fishes of the World 4th ed. Wiley, New Jersey3. Dean MN, Motta PJ (2004) Anatomy and functional morphology

of the feeding apparatus of the lesser electric ray, Narcine brasiliensis (Elasmobranchii: Batoidea). J Morphol 262: 462–483

4. Wilga C (2005) Morphology and evolution of the jaw suspension in Lamniform sharks. J Morphol 265: 102–119

Page 12: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Su

pp

lem

enta

ry T

able

S1.

Li

st o

f spe

cies

ana

lyze

d an

d m

odel

-fitt

ing

resu

lts. F

ish

spec

ies

anal

yzed

as

repr

esen

tativ

es o

f eac

h ex

tant

fish

ord

ers

are

liste

d w

ith b

oth

Lat

in a

nd c

omm

on

nam

es, s

ampl

e si

zes,

Aka

ike

info

rmat

ion

crite

rion

(A

IC)

valu

es o

f the

mod

el fi

tted

to th

e tw

o m

easu

rem

ents

(bo

ld: A

IC o

f the

sel

ecte

d m

odel

), s

ampl

ing

loca

tions

, and

sou

rces

. P-v

alue

s in

LR

T a

re fr

om li

kelih

ood

ratio

test

bet

wee

n FA

mod

el a

nd A

S m

odel

. Com

mon

nam

es fo

llow

thos

e us

ed b

y F

ishB

ase

(htt

p://w

ww

.fish

base

.org

/).T

he in

dex

num

bers

of fi

sh o

rder

s fo

llow

th

e cl

assi

ficat

ion

used

in N

elso

n (2

00

6). P

hylo

gene

tic g

roup

ings

for

tele

osts

in th

is s

tudy

, how

ever

, are

slig

htly

mod

ified

from

Nel

son’

s sy

stem

bas

ed o

n B

etan

cur-

R e

t al.

(201

3).

No.

of

Gro

up in

Sam

ple

|IAS|

|an

gle|

DLI

AIC

in IA

S of

man

dibl

esA

IC in

ang

le b

etw

een

the

head

and

ver

tebr

aeLo

calit

y of

mat

eria

ls

So

urce

of m

ater

ials

orde

r #Fi

g. 3

Ord

er N

ame

Spec

ies N

ame

‡En

glis

h N

ame

size

*m

ean

S.D

.m

ean

S.D

.FA

mod

elD

Am

odel

ASm

odel

LRT

pV o

fA

S to

FA

FAm

odel

DA

mod

elA

Smod

elLR

T pV

of

AS

to F

A

1-

Myx

inifo

rmes

Epta

tret

us b

urge

riIn

shor

e ha

gfis

h11

(5)

3.65

1.09

1.82

0.76

-62

.63

64.2

152.36

< 0.

001

48.1

850

.17

44.42

0.01

6Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

2-

Petro

myz

ontif

orm

esLa

mpe

tra

japo

nica

Arc

tic la

mpr

ey11

(5)

4.11

1.94

4.06

1.28

-66

.52

67.7

664.97

0.06

065

.08

67.0

755.91

< 0.

001

Nor

ther

n Ja

pan

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

3-

Chi

mae

rifor

mes

Chi

mae

ra p

hant

asm

aSi

lver

chi

mae

ra10

(7)

4.82

1.33

2.63

0.85

-62

.58

63.8

551.93

< 0.

001

50.6

852

.21

41.94

0.00

1Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

4-

Het

erod

ontif

orm

esH

eter

odon

tus j

apon

icus

bullh

ead

shar

k10

(4)

3.56

1.51

2.10

0.98

-57

.43

59.3

654.55

0.02

747

.20

48.2

745.79

0.06

5Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

5-

Ore

ctol

obifo

rmes

Chi

losc

ylliu

m p

unct

atum

Bro

wnb

ande

d ba

mbo

osha

rk10

(6)

2.42

0.74

1.77

0.83

-53

.66

55.5

043.77

< 0.

001

47.9

549

.75

46.22

0.05

3Ea

st C

hina

Sea

Cou

rtesy

of D

r. K

. Sat

o (O

kina

wa

Chu

raum

i Aqu

ariu

m)

6-

Lam

nifo

rmes

Lam

na d

itrop

isSa

lmon

shar

k11

(1)

4.08

2.03

2.74

0.86

-66

.58

62.02

65.8

90.

101

56.4

250

.43

47.04

< 0.

001

The

Paci

fic o

f eas

tern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

7-

Car

char

hini

form

esM

uste

lus g

rise

usSp

otle

ss sm

ooth

-hou

nd11

(3)

2.86

0.86

1.69

0.57

-57

.30

55.3

947.08

< 0.

001

45.9

845

.39

38.13

0.00

2Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

8-

Hex

anch

iform

esH

eptr

anch

ias p

erlo

Shar

pnos

e se

veng

ill sh

ark#

5 (5

)5.

241.

312.

420.

35-

--

--

--

--

East

Chi

na S

eaPr

eser

ved

in H

okka

ido

Uni

vers

ity M

useu

m9

-Ec

hino

rhin

iform

esEc

hino

rhin

us b

rucu

sB

ram

ble

shar

k#1

(1)

3.46

-1.

80-

--

--

--

--

-So

uth

Afr

ica

Pres

erve

d in

Hok

kaid

o U

nive

rsity

Mus

eum

10-

Squa

lifor

mes

Squa

lus a

cant

hias

Pike

d do

gfis

h10

(3)

3.61

1.30

1.21

0.53

-57

.25

57.3

651.54

0.00

535

.94

37.2

933.67

0.03

9Th

e Pa

cific

of e

aste

rnPu

rcha

sed

from

fish

erm

an11

-Sq

uatin

iform

esSq

uatin

a ja

poni

caJa

pane

se a

ngel

shar

k10

(5)

3.43

1.44

2.63

1.10

-56

.64

58.2

753.52

0.02

451

.34

53.3

448.03

0.02

1Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

12-

Pris

tioph

orifo

rmes

Pris

tioph

orus

japo

nicu

sJa

pane

se sa

wsh

ark#

5 (1

)4.

621.

433.

190.

85-

--

--

--

--

East

Chi

na S

eaPr

eser

ved

in H

okka

ido

Uni

vers

ity M

useu

m13

-To

rped

inifo

rmes

Nar

ke ja

poni

caJa

pane

se sl

eepe

r ray

11 (4

)3.

491.

371.

380.

66-

62.2

762

.91

57.33

0.00

842

.54

42.4

941.30

0.07

2Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

14-

Pris

tifor

mes

Pris

tis m

icro

don

Larg

etoo

th sa

wfis

h#1

(0)

3.69

-1.

76-

--

--

--

--

-Th

e co

ast o

f Aus

tralia

Pres

erve

d in

KA

IYU

KA

N A

quar

ium

15-

Raj

iform

esO

kam

ejei

ken

ojei

Oce

llate

spot

skat

e10

(4)

3.36

1.33

1.13

0.47

-56

.08

58.0

851.90

0.01

334

.43

35.0

831.31

0.02

4Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

16-

Myl

ioba

tifor

mes

Das

yatis

aka

jei

Japa

nese

stin

gray

80 (3

9)3.

862.

000.

950.

51-

464.

0546

4.45

449.51

< 0.

001

241.

2724

2.07

232.96

0.00

1Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

172

Poly

pter

iform

esPo

lypt

erus

end

liche

riSa

ddle

d bi

chir

10 (7

)4.

741.

112.

030.

763.

735

62.0

461

.84

48.24

< 0.

001

45.8

747

.15

40.82

0.00

8La

ke T

anga

nyik

a &

oth

erC

olle

cted

by

ours

elve

s & P

urch

ased

from

pet

shop

18-

Aci

pens

erifo

rmes

Acip

ense

r sp

.St

urge

on12

(7)

4.42

1.74

2.23

0.95

-73

.36

74.1

867.02

0.00

457

.26

59.0

452.65

0.01

0W

este

rn Ja

pan

Purc

hase

d fr

om fa

rmer

192

Lepi

sost

eifo

rmes

Atra

ctos

teus

spat

ula

Alli

gato

r gar

10 (6

)4.

751.

833.

061.

074.

691

62.9

064

.65

58.30

0.01

053

.92

55.0

847.66

0.00

4So

uth

Am

eric

aPu

rcha

sed

from

pet

shop

202

Am

iifor

mes

Amia

cal

vaB

owfin

10 (5

)4.

951.

123.

040.

914.

741

62.8

764

.85

48.52

< 0.

001

53.4

455

.23

44.29

< 0.

001

Geo

rgia

, USA

Cou

rtesy

of D

r. M

. Kib

bey

& P

urch

ased

from

pet

shop

213

Hio

dont

iform

esH

iodo

n te

rgis

usM

oone

ye10

(4)

4.86

1.36

2.76

0.58

4.43

762

.77

64.4

352.41

< 0.

001

51.1

252

.91

35.37

< 0.

001

Ohi

o, U

SAC

ourte

sy o

f Dr.

M. K

ibbe

y (O

hio

Stat

e U

nive

rsity

)22

3O

steo

glos

sifo

rmes

Ost

eogl

ossu

mA

row

ana

10 (3

)5.

751.

251.

750.

743.

992

65.8

264

.75

50.65

< 0.

001

43.1

642

.69

40.09

0.02

4B

rasi

lPu

rcha

sed

from

pet

shop

233

Elop

iform

esM

egal

ops c

ypri

noid

esIn

do-P

acifi

c ta

rpon

10 (3

)4.

801.

972.

150.

703.

862

63.3

162

.63

59.75

0.01

846

.72

46.3

139.13

0.00

2C

ultu

red

in S

outh

east

Asi

aPu

rcha

sed

from

pet

shop

243

Alb

ulifo

rmes

Albu

la v

irga

taB

onef

ish

10 (6

)2.

611.

082.

110.

563.

021

51.1

453

.10

47.72

0.02

046

.04

47.4

834.80

< 0.

001

Haw

aii,

USA

Cou

rtesy

of D

r. A

. Ros

site

r (W

aiki

ki A

quar

ium

)25

3A

ngui

llifo

rmes

Angu

illa

japo

nica

Japa

nese

eel

10 (7

)4.

781.

181.

810.

723.

576

62.2

662

.81

49.54

< 0.

001

43.6

744

.29

39.73

0.01

5W

este

rn o

f Jap

anC

olle

cted

by

ours

elve

s & P

urch

ase

from

fish

ery

26D

eep

Sacc

opha

ryng

iform

eEu

ryph

aryn

xPe

lican

eel

#1

(0)

11.0

7-

3.24

-7.

601

--

--

--

--

The

Paci

fic o

f sou

ther

nC

ourte

sy o

f JA

MST

EC27

3C

lupe

iform

esC

lupe

a ha

reng

usA

tlant

ic h

errin

g10

(3)

6.29

2.81

1.62

0.63

4.18

168

.97

69.7

466.65

0.03

841

.47

41.9

137.12

0.01

2N

orw

ayPu

rcha

sed

from

fish

erm

an28

3G

onor

ynch

iform

esG

onor

ynch

us a

bbre

viat

usB

ighe

ad b

eake

d sa

ndfis

h11

(7)

5.21

1.47

2.18

1.01

4.06

370

.37

71.9

259.01

< 0.

001

52.5

253

.97

50.57

0.04

7Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves &

Pur

chas

e fr

om fi

sher

y29

3C

yprin

iform

esC

ypri

nus c

arpi

oC

omm

on c

arp

10 (5

)6.

062.

761.

990.

844.

308

68.2

970

.28

66.42

0.04

945

.81

47.3

342.74

0.02

4W

este

rn Ja

pan

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

303

Cha

raci

form

esPy

goce

ntru

s nat

tere

riR

ed p

iranh

a10

(6)

3.54

1.30

1.83

0.57

3.06

456

.90

57.5

451.44

0.00

643

.36

44.4

135.13

0.00

1So

uth

Am

eric

aPu

rcha

sed

from

pet

shop

313

Silu

rifor

mes

Silu

rus a

sotu

sFa

r Eas

tern

cat

fish

110

(53)

6.00

3.65

1.61

0.91

4.02

774

2.15

743.

06736.88

0.00

744

9.28

450.

12439.73

< 0.

001

Wes

tern

of J

apan

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

323

Gym

notif

orm

esAp

tero

notu

s alb

ifron

sB

lack

gho

st10

(4)

5.72

1.26

1.88

0.64

4.06

765

.74

67.7

350.88

< 0.

001

43.5

545

.54

30.89

< 0.

001

Cul

ture

d in

Sou

thea

st A

sia

Purc

hase

d fr

om p

et sh

op33

3A

rgen

tinifo

rmes

Glo

ssan

odon

Dee

psea

smel

t10

(5)

4.42

1.76

1.55

0.65

3.21

361

.58

63.3

557.48

0.01

440

.78

42.5

337.71

0.02

4Se

a of

Japa

nPu

rcha

sed

from

fish

erm

an34

3O

smer

iform

esPl

ecog

loss

us a

ltive

lisA

yu12

(5)

4.43

1.21

2.28

0.67

3.82

572

.66

74.5

759.29

< 0.

001

56.7

758

.45

44.98

< 0.

001

Wes

tern

Japa

nC

olle

cted

by

ours

elve

s35

3Sa

lmon

iform

esO

ncor

hync

hus k

eta

Chu

m sa

lmon

10 (2

)5.

281.

691.

970.

603.

928

64.6

263

.83

56.78

0.00

244

.82

44.4

536.11

0.00

1N

orth

ern

Japa

nPu

rcha

sed

from

fish

erm

an36

3Es

ocifo

rmes

Esox

am

eric

anus

Red

fin p

icke

rel

10 (4

)6.

351.

392.

040.

684.

482

67.8

068

.84

52.84

< 0.

001

45.7

147

.21

38.71

0.00

3O

hio,

USA

Cou

rtesy

of D

r. M

. Kib

bey

(Ohi

o St

ate

Uni

vers

ity)

37D

eep

Stom

iifor

mes

Sigm

ops g

raci

leSl

ende

r fan

gjaw

11 (5

)6.

761.

262.

551.

215.

0575

.64

77.3

955.46

< 0.

001

56.0

656

.17

54.53

0.06

0Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Cou

rtesy

of J

AM

STEC

38D

eep

Ate

leop

odifo

rmes

Atel

eopu

s jap

onic

usPa

cific

jelly

nose

fish

12 (1

0)3.

671.

251.

920.

613.

197

68.5

662

.36

59.99

0.00

152

.90

50.7

742.94

< 0.

001

The

Paci

fic o

f sou

ther

nC

olle

cted

by

ours

elve

s39

4A

ulop

iform

esAu

lopu

s jap

onic

usJa

pane

se th

read

-sai

l fis

h12

(6)

3.38

1.57

1.73

0.80

2.91

67.6

269

.11

65.38

0.03

951

.58

53.4

649.18

0.03

6Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

40D

eep

Myc

toph

iform

esD

iaph

us w

atas

eiW

atas

es la

nter

nfis

h10

(6)

5.22

1.62

4.01

1.68

5.82

64.3

466

.05

55.90

0.00

159

.76

61.3

156.50

0.02

2Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

414

Lam

prid

iform

esLa

mpr

is g

utta

tus

Opa

h10

(3)

2.91

1.72

2.11

0.80

3.11

354

.75

51.71

56.0

80.

413

46.6

746

.45

41.74

0.00

8Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

424

Poly

mix

iifor

mes

Poly

mix

ia ja

poni

caSi

lver

eye

10 (5

)2.

881.

122.

661.

243.

694

52.9

554

.35

48.49

0.01

151

.91

53.7

550.47

0.06

4Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

434

Perc

opsi

form

esPe

rcop

sis o

mis

com

aycu

sTr

out-p

erch

10 (6

)4.

471.

082.

130.

823.

707

60.9

062

.44

47.86

< 0.

001

46.9

348

.12

42.37

0.01

0O

hio,

USA

Cou

rtesy

of D

r. M

. Kib

bey

(Ohi

o St

ate

Uni

vers

ity)

444

Gad

iform

esG

adus

mac

roce

phal

usPa

cific

cod

10 (7

)2.

880.

962.

250.

923.

251

52.5

853

.57

45.39

0.00

248

.11

48.0

744.65

0.01

9Th

e Pa

cific

of n

orth

ern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

454

Oph

idiif

orm

esSi

rem

bo im

berb

isG

olde

n cu

sk10

(6)

3.34

1.00

2.41

1.02

3.56

155

.35

55.3

346.24

< 0.

001

49.5

951

.56

46.62

0.02

6Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves &

Pur

chas

ed fr

om fi

sher

y46

4B

atra

choi

difo

rmes

Batr

acho

moe

usTh

ree-

spin

ed to

adfis

h10

(3)

6.52

1.46

1.84

0.60

4.43

368

.35

67.5

253.81

< 0.

001

43.5

844

.47

36.08

0.00

2So

uthe

ast A

sia

Purc

hase

d fr

om p

et sh

op47

Dee

pLo

phiif

orm

esLo

phiu

s litu

lon

Ang

lerf

ish

12 (8

)7.

842.

533.

411.

156.

222

86.5

885

.94

75.97

< 0.

001

66.7

264

.99

57.13

< 0.

001

The

Paci

fic o

f sou

ther

nC

olle

cted

by

ours

elve

s & P

urch

ased

from

fish

ery

485

Mug

ilifo

rmes

Mug

il ce

phal

usFl

athe

ad m

ulle

t11

(4)

4.40

1.51

2.35

0.87

3.87

867

.02

68.7

059.45

0.00

253

.44

53.2

847.41

0.00

5W

este

rn Ja

pan

Cou

rtesy

of D

r.M. N

akam

ura

(Cor

bicu

la R

esea

rch

Inst

itute

)49

5A

ther

inifo

rmes

Athe

rion

ely

mus

Bea

rded

silv

ersi

de10

(6)

4.17

0.88

3.08

0.86

4.52

59.3

561

.09

43.77

< 0.

001

53.5

955

.58

43.16

< 0.

001

The

Paci

fic o

f sou

ther

nC

olle

cted

by

ours

elve

s50

5B

elon

iform

esSt

rong

ylur

a an

asto

mel

laN

eedl

efis

h10

(8)

3.85

1.14

1.96

0.66

3.30

558

.15

56.4

048.82

< 0.

001

44.8

840

.44

38.01

0.00

3W

este

rn Ja

pan

Cou

rtesy

of D

r.M. N

akam

ura

(Cor

bicu

la R

esea

rch

Inst

itute

)50

-2-

Bel

onifo

rmes

Prog

nich

thys

bre

vipi

nnis

Shor

tfin

flyin

gfis

h10

(6)

4.28

0.92

1.30

0.27

-59

.93

61.6

344.66

< 0.

001

36.0

537

.67

20.01

< 0.

001

The

Paci

fic o

f sou

ther

nPu

rcha

sed

from

fish

erm

an51

5C

yprin

odon

tifor

mes

Gam

busi

a af

finis

Mos

quito

fish

10 (5

)5.

130.

731.

710.

233.

665

63.2

964

.66

39.93

< 0.

001

41.2

443

.23

16.62

< 0.

001

Wes

tern

Japa

nC

olle

cted

by

ours

elve

s52

Dee

pSt

epha

nobe

ryci

form

Poro

mitr

a cr

assi

ceps

Cre

sted

big

scal

e10

(6)

4.96

1.13

4.81

2.25

6.61

562

.91

64.7

748.69

< 0.

001

63.7

565

.56

62.23

0.06

1Th

e Pa

cific

of n

orth

ern

Pres

erve

d in

Hok

kaid

o U

nive

rsity

Mus

eum

53D

eep

Ber

ycifo

rmes

Bery

x sp

lend

ens

Sple

ndid

alfo

nsin

o11

(6)

2.97

1.25

2.92

1.42

4.00

553

.76

53.4

350.56

0.02

353

.90

55.1

652.69

0.07

3Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

544

Zeifo

rmes

Zeus

fabe

rJo

hn d

ory

12 (6

)2.

200.

632.

711.

233.

589

55.8

957

.89

43.77

< 0.

001

62.1

964

.09

59.54

0.03

1Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

555

Gas

tero

stei

form

esG

aste

rost

eus

sp.

Thre

e-sp

ined

stic

kleb

ack

10 (5

)6.

902.

021.

520.

384.

443

69.8

371

.81

60.34

< 0.

001

39.3

841

.35

26.91

< 0.

001

Nor

ther

n Ja

pan

(the

Sea

ofC

ourte

sy o

f Dr.

S. M

ori (

Gifu

Eco

nom

y U

nive

rsity

)56

5Sy

nbra

nchi

form

esM

asta

cem

belu

s moo

rii

Spin

y ee

l12

(3)

3.82

1.36

1.95

0.41

3.28

469

.66

70.4

262.13

0.00

252

.63

51.9

133.30

< 0.

001

Lake

Tan

gany

ika

Col

lect

ed b

y ou

rsel

ves

575

Scor

paen

iform

esSe

bast

iscu

s mar

mor

atus

Fals

e ke

lpfis

h10

(5)

3.51

0.81

2.17

0.85

3.37

656

.06

58.0

242.05

< 0.

001

47.3

249

.32

43.13

0.01

3Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

585

Perc

iform

esLa

teol

abra

x ja

poni

cus

Japa

nese

seap

erch

11 (3

)4.

461.

401.

600.

523.

269

67.1

465

.32

57.92

< 0.

001

44.5

843

.90

35.89

0.00

1Th

e Pa

cific

of s

outh

ern

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

58-2

-Se

riol

a qu

inqu

erad

iata

Japa

nese

am

berja

ck10

0 (4

4)2.

831.

641.

510.

72-

523.

0851

9.15

516.29

0.00

338

8.44

388.

68359.58

< 0.

001

Sea

of Ja

pan

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

58-3

-Pe

riss

odus

mic

role

pis

L. T

anga

nyik

a sc

ale-

eate

r10

(3)

9.63

1.56

1.99

0.41

-75

.86

76.5

950.74

< 0.

001

45.1

544

.84

37.51

0.00

2La

ke T

anga

nyik

aC

olle

cted

by

ours

elve

s59

5Pl

euro

nect

iform

esH

ippo

glos

soid

es d

ubiu

sFl

athe

ad fl

ound

er50

(28)

4.11

3.32

2.23

1.05

3.65

5310.37

312.

2631

2.37

1.00

023

4.13

234.

55219.42

< 0.

001

Wes

tern

Japa

nPu

rcha

sed

from

fish

erm

an59

-2-

Para

licht

hys o

livac

eus

Oliv

e flo

unde

r51

(21)

4.55

4.00

2.78

1.62

-330.43

330.

8533

2.43

1.00

026

6.02

265.

99263.35

0.03

1W

este

rn Ja

pan

Purc

hase

d fr

om fi

sher

man

605

Tetra

odon

tifor

mes

Taki

fugu

rubr

ipes

Japa

nese

puf

ferf

ish

10 (4

)3.

910.

932.

890.

784.

2458

.18

60.0

044.77

< 0.

001

52.2

853

.94

41.38

< 0.

001

Wes

tern

Japa

nPu

rcha

sed

from

fish

erm

an61

1C

oela

cant

hifo

rmes

Latim

eria

cha

lum

nae

Coe

laca

nth#

2 (2

)4.

200.

134.

330.

815.

889

--

--

--

--

Coa

st o

f Tan

zani

aPr

eser

ved

in T

okyo

Inst

itute

of T

echn

olog

y62

1C

erat

odon

tifor

mes

Prot

opte

rus a

ethi

opic

usM

arbl

ed lu

ngfis

h11

(5)

3.68

1.43

2.88

1.21

4.15

963

.42

64.6

658.40

0.00

858

.24

59.8

254.54

0.01

7Ea

st A

fric

aC

olle

cted

by

ours

elve

s & P

urch

ased

from

pet

shop

# N

umbe

rs o

f ord

er fo

llow

Nel

son

(200

2).

‡ Sp

ecie

s who

se sa

mpl

es a

re le

ss th

an 1

0.

* Fi

gure

s in

pare

nthe

ses i

ndic

ate

the

num

ber o

f rig

hty

indi

vidu

als i

n th

e sa

mpl

es.

Page 13: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Supplementary Figure S1. Classification and phylogenetic tree of the extant orders of teleost fishes used in this study. Phylogenetic trees are modified following Betancur-R et al. (2013). The classification is based on Nelson (2006), with a slight modification following Betancur-R et al. (2013) as shown in Supplementary Table 1 (colors are the same as those in Fig. 3A). The blue plot shows deep-sea fishes.

Page 14: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Supplementary Figure S2. Laterality of the Japanese stingray (Dasyatis akajei). (A, B) Cartilaginous bones showing the data points used for measurements. (A) Ventral view of the jaw arch and a close-up of the left mandibular cartilage showing the data points for the index of asymmetry (IAS) of the mandibles. The arrow indicates the height of the mandible at the posterior end (HMPE), which is the distance from the postero-dorsal edge of the sustentaculum in the mandibular cartilage (PE) to the ventral corner of the mandibular cartilage (VC). In car-tilaginous fishes (except in chimaeras), the sustentaculum functions as the mandibular surface of the hyomandibular-mandibular joint, and the nodule on the VC functions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis (Dean and Motta, 2004; Wilga 2005), such that the HMPE provides a functional measure equivalent to that of teleost fishes. (B) Ventral view of head and vertebrae showing data points for the head angle (θ ), which is the angle between the head and vertebrae on the ventral side of the anterior part of the skeleton. CC: the small fora-men near the center of the cranial cartilage; PC: the midline of the postero-ventral margin of the cranium; PV: the first large process on the ventral sagittal line of the vertebra. (C, D) Frequency distributions of the (C) IAS of the mandibles and (D) θ in the Japanese stingray. Lines show the probability densities derived from the three models (see Methods). The model selected by the lowest Akaike information criterion (AIC) value is indicated by the thick line.

Page 15: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Supplementary Figure S3. Relationship between growth and laterality in large-mouth bass (Micropterus salmoides). The absolute values of the index of asymmetry (IAS) of mandibles and the head angles (θ ) in three size classes did not differ sig-nificantly (ANOVA, P = 0.2379 for IAS and P = 0.7791 for θ ). Individuals were sam-pled from the same population in Lake Biwa, Japan, in 2004–2008. Vertical bars indicate standard deviations (s.d.). Small fish were primarily 0 + (n = 20, standard length [SL; mean ± s.d.] = 76.1 ± 4.1 mm), medium fish were primarily 1+ (n = 20, SL = 125.1 ± 13.6 mm), and large fish were primarily ≥ 2 + (n = 20, SL = 173.4 ± 16.8 mm).

Supplementary Figure S4. Laterality of the flatfishes. Frequency distributions of the (A) index of symmetry (IAS) of the mandibles and (B) head angles in flathead flounder (Hippoglossoides dubius; upper) and olive flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus; lower). The lines are the same as those used in Fig. 2. In both species, the model with the best fit to the distributions of IAS was fluctuating asymmetry, whereas the best model for head angle was anti-symmetry (see Supplementary Table 1).

Page 16: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Supplementary Figure S5. Laterality of the hagfish. (A, B) Data for measurements from inshore hagfish (Eptatretus burgeri). (A) Ventral view of the mouthparts of a moderately boiled fish sample. A pair of flat cartilaginous structures behind the tooth plates is exposed. FC: the length of the flat cartilaginous structure used for the index of asymmetry (IAS). (B) Dorsal view of the anterior part of the body in a transparent preparation showing data for the head angle. CN: the center of the cartilaginous rings of the nasal tube; MC: the posterior end of the mem-branous cranium; DP: the dorsal datum point equidistant from MC to CN–MC on the sagittal line. Head angle (θ ) is defined as the angle of the line extending anteriorly from MC–DP to CN–MC. A fish with a clockwise positive angle is defined as left type, whereas a fish with a negative angle is defined as right type. The definitions of right and left types are consistent with those shown in the ventral view of Fig. 1. (C, D) Frequency distributions of (C) IAS of the flat cartilaginous structure and (D) θ in inshore hagfish. The lines are the same as those used in Fig. 2. Both distributions showed anti-symmetry (see Supplementary Table 1).

Page 17: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Supplementary Figure S6. Laterality of the lamprey. (A, B) Data points used for measurements in arctic lamprey (Lampetra japonica). (A) Ventral view of a mouthpart in which a pair of spinose cartilaginous structures behind the suction cup are exposed in a moderately boiled fish sample. SC: the length of the spinose cartilage used for the index of asymmetry (IAS). (B) Dorsal view of the anterior part of the body in a transparent specimen showing the data points used for head angle (θ ) on the ventral sagittal line. PD: the anterior end of the posterior dorsal cartilage; PO: the posterior end of the postero-occipital cartilaginous ring; DP: the dorsal datum point equidistant to PD–PO. θ is defined as the angle from the line extending anteriorly from PO–DP to PD–PO. A fish with a clockwise positive angle is defined as left type, whereas a fish with a negative angle is defined as right type. (C, D) Frequency distributions of the (C) IAS of spinose cartilaginous structures and (D) θ in arctic lamprey. The lines are the same as those in Fig. 2. Both distributions showed anti-symmetry (see Supplementary Table 1).

Page 18: Laterality is Universal Among Fishes but Increasingly Cryptic ...behav.zoology.unibe.ch/sysuif/uploads/files/Hori_et_al...tions as the attachment point of the depressor mandibularis

Supplementary Figure S7. Degrees of laterality in extant cartilaginous fish orders and sturgeons. Two measures of the laterality, the index of asymmetry (IAS) of the mandibles and the head angle, are displayed as in Fig. 3A. Fish species are grouped into four color-coded categories.