10
Totem: The University of Western Ontario Journal of Anthropology Volume 10,  Issue 1 2002  Article 9 A Proto-Human Language: Fact or Fiction Christine Schreyer The University of Western Ontario Copyright  c 2002 by the authors.  Totem: The Univer sity of Western Ontari o Journal of Anthro-  pology  is produced by The Berkeley Electronic Press (bepress). http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/totem

Proto Human Language

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 1/10

Totem: The University of Western

Ontario Journal of Anthropology 

Volume 10,  Issue 1 2002   Article 9

A Proto-Human Language: Fact or Fiction

Christine Schreyer∗

∗The University of Western Ontario

Copyright   c2002 by the authors.   Totem: The University of Western Ontario Journal of Anthro-

 pology  is produced by The Berkeley Electronic Press (bepress). http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/totem

Page 2: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 2/10

S chrryer -   Pro to-Human Language   53

A Proto-Human Language: Fact or Fiction?

The question of where and when

anatomically modem humans emerged has

long been a controversial issue in

anthropology.   Evidence from many

different disciplines has been increasingly

useful in attempting to pinpoint the

origins of Homo sapiens in time and  place.   As it is suggested that anatomical

modems were the first humans to have

the cognitive capacity for modem

linguistic capabilities (Lieberman 1984), it

may be plausible to use linguistic evidence

in analyzing this biological evolution.

This is because one of the main tenets of 

historical linguistics is that "languages

spread as people move" (Barbujani and 

Pilastro 1993). In fact,   Dolukhanov

writes in his examination of thearchaeological record that:

The advent of language in the

Upper Paleolithic was a powerful

factor that through the

intensification of information

transfer both in time (from one

generation to the next) and space

(between neighbouring groups)

drastically accelerated social and 

cultural evolution.   (1993).

The purpose of this paper is to determine

whether or not linguistics can truly be

used to uncover the origins of anatomical

modem humans, or if there is a time limit

at which linguistic comparisons can no

longer be made. This timeframe within

which linguistic comparisons can be

established will also be discussed with

consideration of Proto-Human, and the

idea that "all the languages spoken on

earth (roughly 5,000...) are descendant of a

single ancestral language" (Ruhlen 1994b).

This paper will describe comparativelinguistics, and the many controversies

which are present in this type of study as

well as give examples of linguistic familieswhich have been compiled. It will also

look at genetic evidence, and the ability of 

this to support the linguistic data.

Examples for and against the ability for 

comparative linguistics to compile a

 proto-language that encompasses all

human languages will also be discussed in

an attempt to determine how far these

studies are viable into the past.

Comparative linguistics, sometimes

called historical linguistics, is often used in

attempts to link languages that have

similar cognates, phonology, and  

grammatical structures together. These

languages which are grouped together are

called language families, and are said to be

linked genetically. The languages which

are related are then combined into proto-

languages, or languages which are"reconstructed out of the evidence that is

acquired by the careful comparison of the

daughter languages [or lower level

languages]" (Haas 1978). These proto-

languages are only estimations of original

languages, however, because it is

impossible to know exactly which

characteristics from each daughter 

Schreyer: A Proto-Human Language: Fact or Fiction

Produced by The Berkeley Electronic Press, 2002

Page 3: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 3/10

language were present in the original

language. Proto-languages of spoken or 

recorded languages are fairly accurate

although estimations which move further 

away from the actual data are more

distorted, and are therefore lessdemonstrable. This is one of the major 

 problems with comparative linguistics of 

deep-timeframes, and is an essential fact

to remember when attempting to compile

an original languagewhich may have been

ancestral to all languages on earth

although advocates of this theory such as

Greenberg and Ruhlen do not seem to

realize this. Another problem with

historical linguistics is the lack of an

absolute chronology.Colin Renfrew writes that,   "there

seems nothing, however, in the pattern of 

linguistic change which would allow for 

the establishment of any kind of absolute

chronology" (1990). He suggests that

linguists take it for granted that languages

change at a constant rate, and that

theories based on this premise such as

glottochronology are erroneous in nature.

Glottochronology was a formula originally

developed by Morris Swadesh in the1960's as a means to "predict the history

of the derivation from a common

ancestor of a group of interrelated 

languages reduced to sample wordlists"

(Guy 1980). It has fallen under universal

criticism, however, partially as a result of 

"too much theorizing and little

experimenting" (Guy 1980). This still

leaves comparative linguistics without a

model for chronology although there have

 been many attempts to find one. There isalso the question of   w~   languageswould 

diverge at a constant rate when there are

numerous reasons why they would not

such as geographical location and 

isolation, population densities, and the

state of relations within the speech

community.

S chrryer - Pro to- H um an L anguage   54

Another problem in the field of 

comparative linguistics is the basic

assumption that comparisons "account for 

similarities which cannot be attributed to

chance"(Hock and Joseph 1996), and are

therefore the result of a commonancestor. The controversy stems from a

 belief that the cognates which

comparative linguists use are not a

 product of related languages,but are due

to the processes of diffusion, borrowing,

and onomatopoeia. These aspects do

occur, but are relativelyeasy to identifyas

chance rather than the result of genetic

relations while lexical similarities which

are systematic and recurring are more

likely to be due to relations as are the presence of idiosyncrasies in languages

(Hock and Joseph 1996). Borrowing is

the use of words from one language into

another, and "will generally be found in

language families that are geographically

adjacent...(though prehistoric adjacency

might be hard to judge)"(Nettle 1999). It

stands to reason then that languages that

are geographically far apart will have less

of a chance of havingsimilarwords unless

they truly are related. Borrowing alsotends to be limited to certain parts of 

vocabulary which are often technical in

nature. In contrast similar words which

are part of any basic vocabulary are likely

to be the result of genetic relationships

(Hock and Joseph 1996). Onomatopoeic

expressions or sounds of nature also tend 

to be remarkably similar in languages

which are not related as are "nursery

words" or words "which adults assign to

the early babbling of infants" (Hock and Joseph 1996).

The higher the number of 

languages which can be compared results

in the probability of fewer chance

similarities. However ,   using comparisons

of too many languages can also be

 problematic as is seen in the criticisms of 

Joseph Greenberg's methods of mass

Totem: The University of Western Ontario Journal of Anthropology, Vol. 10 [2002], Iss. 1, Art. 9

http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/totem/vol10/iss1/9

Page 4: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 4/10

comparisons to create exceptionally large

 proto-languages from families of 

languages which are combined into lower 

level p roto-languages. The mass

comparison or multi-lateral comparison

method establishes genetic affinities of languages through comparison of a large

number of languages on a fairly limited 

 basis (Hock and Joseph 1996). This is

related to one last criticism of comparative

linguistics which is that much of the work 

has been completed by one highly

dedicated individual, and for work to be

more widely accepted it is necessary for 

movement beyond "the great scholar"

(NettIe 1999). This is partially due to the

 biases which individual researchers may bring to their comparisons. People with

different knowledge may come up with

different results even when using the same

methodology (NettIe 1999).

EXAMPLES OF PROTO-

LANGUAGE

Greenberg has used his multilateral

comparison method in the midst of much

criticism to suggest that the Americas arecompiled of three different linguistic

groups each of which coincides with a

separate migration into the New World.

Since traditional linguists suggest that

comparisons of languages cannot be made

realistically past 7,000 years they eliminate

the possibility of genetic affmity between

American languages and populations with

Asian languages and populations as this

 predates the arrival of people into the

 New World. Greenberg's comparisonsnegate this notion, and support the

relationship between the two groups of 

 people. The first migration which he

suggests took place approximately 11,000-

12,000 years ago, and coincides with the

 proto-language of Amerind. Amerind is

 believed to be the oldest language

migration into North America because it

5 ch rry e r - P ro to -Human L anguage   55

has not only reached the furthest

geographical regions from which it came,

 but also because it "shows greater internal

differentiation" (Greenberg, Turner, and 

Zegura 1986). The second migration is

the arrival of people who speak Na-Dene,and who populated mostly the North

West coast of North America,   and are

therefore geographically less peripheral

than the third language group of Eskimo-

Aleut. The arrival of the Na-Dene

language group appears to be

approximately 5, 000 years B.P. (Krauss

1973). The third migration which is

 probably the most recent around 4,000 B.

P. is the one which coincides with the

linguistic family of Eskimo-Aleut,   and whose populations live in the

 Northernmost part of North America.

The languages and genes of this linguistic

group have been shown to have

relationships with Asian populations.

Amerind is the most controversial

of the groupings, and this is partially due

to the fact that it contains the highest

amount of language families. As well it is

thought to be of the greatest antiquity.

There is also the controversy over the factthat Greenberg uses oral languages which

have not all been extensively researched to

complete his comparison. Arguments

against his method include the idea of 

Pan-Americanism which is the belief that

there is a similarity of words in North

American languages simply due to

similarities between Native cultures. As

well, the general belief which most

linguists hold is that there is simply too

much to be compared, and thatGreenberg can not thoroughly compare

all of the languages which are classified as

Amerind. Critics often point out errors in

transcription and other minor details to

suggest that Amerind is a poor quality

comparison although few have the

evidence which would suggest the whole

work was not credible. In fact Greenberg

Schreyer: A Proto-Human Language: Fact or Fiction

Produced by The Berkeley Electronic Press, 2002

Page 5: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 5/10

summanzes his article   In Difense   if 

 Amerind    by quoting Lamb who writes,

"The volume calls for careful study and 

for follow-up research. It does not call

for criticism based on incomplete

understanding of his methods" (1987).Another proto-language which is

on the same levelas Amerind is the proto-

language of Nostratic which is a term that

is somewhat ethnocentric meaning

"belonging to us/to our part of the world 

i.e. Eurasia plus Northern Africa" (Hock 

and Joseph 1996). The validity of 

 Nostratic, like Amerind, is also not

accepted by the majority of historical

linguistics,but "if it is not proven, it is not

discredited either" (Nettle 1999). Again, itis the lack of a concrete methodology

which gives issue to the controversy over 

 Nostratic, and similarly to Amerind,

 Nostratic's biggest critics are convinced 

that the Proto-language contains too many

languages, and that the similarities

 between them are just random.   However,

unlike Amerind the languagesof Nostratic

are spread across many different regions,

and because the origins of people in these

regions are not the result of a migration infixed time and place like the arrival of 

 people into the New World critics of 

 Nostratic claim that the combination of 

the languagefamiliesis less accurate.,   The

original compilation of Nostratic was also

completed by one scholar, Aharon

Dolgopolsky, and this is also cause for 

concern as many other linguists including

Lyle Campbell find serious problems with

"nearly all of Dogopolsky's 124 Nostratic

lexical sets" (1999). There are still manydoubts about the soundness of Nostratic,

and unless it can be supported with more

evidence linguists will continue to have

these doubts, and "any interpretations of 

non-linguistic prehistory based on it

remain sheer speculation" (Campbell

1999). Use of non-linguistic sources to

corroborate these linguistic comparisons

Schrryer- Proto-Human lAnguage 56 

may be the way to eliminate these doubts

from the minds of many scholars

especiallywhen these other sources might

 provide concrete chronologies and 

observable changes throughout evolution

of people which historical linguistics cannot see. Raimo Antilla writes that

"collaboration between archaeologists and 

linguists is desirable because each side

tends to apply the findings of the other 

too simplistically"(1989).

For a long period of time there

has been the hope that "evidence from

molecular genetics will cast more lightupon population histories which may in

turn have a bearing on language history

also" (Renfrew 1998). The use of genetic

evidence for linguistic comparisons is

discussed in this section. Genes and 

languages have many similar traits in

common when it comes to the way in

which they spread, and the ways in which

they are studied consequently. Both

utilize tree diagrams in an attempt to

relate the genes/languages of today back to an original source as well as show the

"representation of inherited relationships"

(McMahon, Lohr, and McMahon 1999).

One fundamental difference which

McMahon, Lohr ,   and McMahon note in

their article "Family Trees and Favorite

Daughters" is that biologistsuse trees that

are more based on quantitative data while

linguists work more intuitively(1999).

Cavalli-Sforza has been the main

 biologist who has done studieswhich link these two separate phenomenon together.

He suggests that the spread of genes and 

languages are linked together, and that

studies of mtDNA enable patterns of 

gene diffusion to be isolated to individual

 populations. Thus, if a gene is in one

 population, and moves to another, but not

to a third then those populations that

Totem: The University of Western Ontario Journal of Anthropology, Vol. 10 [2002], Iss. 1, Art. 9

http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/totem/vol10/iss1/9

Page 6: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 6/10

have the same gene must also have similar 

languages, and consequently cultures.

Cavalli-Sforza also notes that there are

fewer linguistic trees than genetic ones,

 but "it is usually true that the genetic

similarity between populations belonging

to the same linguistic family is high"

(1997).There are a few exceptions to this

rule including the populations of Lapps,

Ethiopians, and Tibetans each of which

speak a language to which they are not

 biologicallylinked. These exceptions may

 be related to gene flow and language

replacement, and while they "blur the

genetic and linguistic picture...they do not

obscure it entirely" (Cavalli-Sforza1990).

Cavalli-Sforza also considers the place of 

origin of the first populations of 

anatomical modems by examining how

closely related populations are to one

another. Cavalli-Sforza indicates his

 believe that further collaboration between

genetics and linguistics will be beneficial

in examining the origins of modem

humans. There are similar problems

within each discipline as the rate at which

genes and languages change is still

controversial, and the rate at which genes

change is one of the main differences

 between the continuity and replacement

models of human origins.

In respect to this genetic evidence

Cavalli-Sforza indicates that all

 populations are related, and that Africa

and Asia are more closely linked than

Europe (1997). As well, one of the

earliest branches in the evolution of 

modem humans is the separation of Mricans and Non-Africans (1997). This

suggests an African Origins Model, and 

would therefore suggest a replacement

model.   Linguistic evidence also suggestsa

replacement model (Ruhlen 1994b) as it is

with the arrival of anatomical modems

that the cognitive capacity for languageis

 present suggesting monogenesis of all

languages. Genetic studies of specific

S c h r ry e r- P ro to - H um an L anguage   57 

areas which link linguistic groups to

 populations are also used in attempting to

 prove comparative linguistic studies.

These include theories by Turner on

dental evidence, and Zegura on genetic

differences such as blood types which

indicate that there were three distinct

migrations into the Americas, and the

 populations of these migrations correlate

with Greenberg's proposal of three

linguistic groups of Eskimo-Aleut, Na-

Dene, and Amerind (Greenberg, Turner,

and Zegura 1986). Similarwork has been

done which connects Nostratic to genetic

 populations of Eurasia and North Africa;

the areas from which the languages of 

 Nostratic are taken (Barbujaniand Pilastro

1993). There have also been

demonstrated genetic and linguistic

 population convergences in Australia

(Ruhlen1994b),and Southeast Asia (Baer 

1995).

FOR AND AGAINST PROTO-

HUMAN

The current view of most

historical linguists is that there is a time-

limit past which languages can not be

reconstructed. This limit has been

suggested at different times which vary

drasticallyfrom 5,000 (Hock and Joseph

1996), to 6,000 (Ruhlen 1994a; Ehret

1999), to 7,000 years (Greenberg 1996)

depending on the study. All this variation

 between different scholars beliefs of when

linguistic comparisons can no longer be

done appears to indicateuncertainty in thefield, and suggests that perhaps the time-

limit is not so earlyas it was once thought

to be. Few linguistswould admit to this,

however, who do not already believe in

the deep-time comparisons of languages

leading to proto-languages such as

 Nostratic and Amerind. Some linguists

such as Ruhlen and Greenberg, as well as

a few others, suggest taking comparisons

Schreyer: A Proto-Human Language: Fact or Fiction

Produced by The Berkeley Electronic Press, 2002

Page 7: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 7/10

of language even further than this to

create what might be known as proto-

human where the "ultimate goal [is] a

comprehensive classification of what is

very likely a single language family"

(Greenberg 1987). The construction of 

this proto-language is situated in the belief that:

Human language

came into being just once and 

that all languages that now

exist (or ever have existed) are

(or were) altered later forms

of this original language"

(Ruhlen 1994b).

Greenberg and Ruhlen have even

suggested 27 global etymologies whichmay in fact be evidence of proto-human,

and Ruhlen suggests that these are only a

fraction of what could actuallybe found if 

closer comparisons were made (Ruhlen

1994a).

It is my belief, however, that

eventually a point is reached where proto-

languages come to the extent of the data

that the languages of present provide, and 

any other comparisons beyond this are

too far away from the original data to becorrect.   To summarize, the words of the

 proto-language are so distorted that it is

impossible to tell whether the connection

 between cognates is a true one or not

 because we will never know if the proto-

languages are even close to languages

which were spoken in the past. This

 belief is supported with data in Hock and 

Joseph's (1996) book which suggest that

the global etymologies of Proto-human as

suggested by Greenberg and Ruhlen are"overly short". This is problematic in the

sense that chance similarities are more

likely to occur in short words than in

longer ones (Hock and Joseph 1996;

Moravcisk 1978). The one etymology

which is not too short is the proto-human

term   maliq'a   meaning throat or swallow

S c h r ry e r- P r o to - H u m a n L a n g ua g e   58 

(Hock and Joseph 1996). Greenberg and 

Ruhlen fmd this cognate in "Amerind,

Eskimo-Aleut,  and four language families

of the Old World: Mro-Asiatic, Indo-

European, Uralic, and Dravidian" (Hock 

and Joseph 1996);it appears in thirty-two

languagesin all.   Even with this suggested 

similarity in many languages numerous

linguistsfind problems with the semantics

of the cognate as they include such

varying meanings as, "swallow, throat,

suck, chew, milk , breast, and neck" (Hock 

and Joseph 1996).

Other indications that these global

etymologies might be incorrect is seen in

the article written by Trask which states

that he can fmd fifteen similarities

 between Basque, a reputed languageisolate, out of the 124 cognates described 

in the proto-language of Nostratic (1999).

If a language which is known for being

isolated linguistically,and genetically (due

to the highest frequency of Rh-negative

gene of any population (Ruhlen 1994b))

can be seen to have similarities with

higher levelproto-languages the legitimacy

of this type of study for deep time-frames

is calledinto question. It is also important

to note that the Basque language survived the migration of Indo-European

languages into its region (Ruhlen 1994b).

This suggests that Basque is the exception

to the rule that languages spread with

 people, and this challengesthe assumption

of language replacement in all territories

except previously non-populated ones

(Renfrew 1990).

Pessimistic views of this sort

which doubt whether there is enough

evidence to support proto-humanlanguages have been around for a long

time. As early as 1867 American historical

linguist William Dwight Whitney wrote,

"Linguisticscience is not now, and cannot

ever hope to be, in a condition to give an

authoritative opinion respecting the unity

or variety of our species". This is not to

Totem: The University of Western Ontario Journal of Anthropology, Vol. 10 [2002], Iss. 1, Art. 9

http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/totem/vol10/iss1/9

Page 8: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 8/10

say that linguistics, archaeology, and 

genetics are not beneficial to each other 

 but the evidence which linguistics can

 provide is set within a limited time-frame.

As Ruhlen writes "we know a good deal

about what our ancestors looked like, and how they lived, but their minds and 

languages remain shrouded in the past"

(1994b).

This paper has discussed what

comparative linguistics are, and the

timeframe in which they can be applied, as

well as why it is highly unlikely that use of 

linguistic evidence can be traced back as

far as the origins of modem humans.

This essay argues that linguistic evidence

is useful, however, in showing that

 populations of today are related, and how

these changes in languages have occurred 

as populations have spread across the

globe. The languages of Amerind are

especially useful to study as they show

how languages spread across a previously

uninhabited continent (according to

archaeological evidence), and at what rate.

Although the true diversity of alllanguages spoken can not really ever be

known approximate rate of change is an

important key to determining how far 

 back languages might be related even if 

they cannot physically be connected. This

essay has also looked at the place of 

genetics in collaboration with linguistics,

and how the combination of the two

 pieces of data provide for a fuller 

understanding of how anatomical

modems spread at later time-periods than15,000 y ears ago. Whether or not

Ruhlen's notion of a proto-human

language is a reality or not is difficult if 

not impossible to determine using the

evidence of the present. I quote Colin

Renfrew's current appraisal of the

Schrryer- Proto-Human Language   59

evidence to date on the use of linguistics

to prove time and place of human origins:

If the lumpers (notably the

Russian school including Dolgopolsky ...,

and the American school of Greenberg

and Ruhlen) are correct, then the unity of human origins may be reflected in the

evidence offered for linguistic

monogenesis ...But if the splitters are right,

and if it is indeed the case that languages

evolve so fast that no reconstructions of 

any kind could be possible beyond about

5,000 years ago ..., then, these broad 

macro-families (e.g. Amerind, Austric,

 Nostratic or Eurasiatic, Sino-Caucasian)

would be entirely illusory, and so too in

consequence would any supposed  

correration between them and the genetic

evidence for human phylogeny ... (1992).

I personally believe that a single origin of 

all human languages does exist,   but trying

to prove its existence is another matter as

is attempting to describe it. The data

eventually gets too thin to postulate a

reliable version of a proto-language which

encompasses all of the world's languages

and consequently the language of the

original anatomically modem humans.

Antilla,Raimo 1989.   Historical   and Compara t ive

 L ing u is ti c s .   Amsterdam and New York 

(NY):Benjamins.

Baer,A. S. 1995. Human Genes and  bicultural history in Southeast Asia.  Asian

P er spec !J :ves .  34(1):21-35.

Barbujani,Guido and Andrea Pilastro 1993.Genetic Evidence on origin and dispersalof human populations speaking languages of the Nostratic Macrofamily.   Froc. Notl.

 Acad. Sci. USA~   90: 4670-73.

Campbell,Lyle 1999. "Nostratic and linguisticpalaeontology in methodological

Perspectives." In   N o st ra ti c: E x a m in in g a

Schreyer: A Proto-Human Language: Fact or Fiction

Produced by The Berkeley Electronic Press, 2002

Page 9: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 9/10

 L in gu is ti c M acro fa m ify .   Colin Renfrew

and David Nettle, (ed.). pp. 179-230.

Cambridge: McDonald Institute for 

Archaeological Research.

Cavalli-Sforza,   L. L.  1997. Genes, Peoples,

and Languages.   From the Colloquiwn:Gen e t ic s a n d On ' gins 0/ the Species .   California:

 National Academy of Sciences. 94: 7719-

24.

--- 1990. Comment.   CumntAnthropology .   31 :

16-18.

Dolukhanov, Paul, M. 1993. "The

Pleistocene-Holocene Boundary:

Environmental Processes and Social

Adaptations." In   From Kos tenki to Clov i s :

U~f f~~~uand~~~~~Ad~~@~Olga Soffer and N. D.   Praslov, (ed.).

 pp.189-196. New York: Plenwn Press.

Ehret, Christopher 1999. "Nostratic or 

Proto-human?"   I n No s tra t ic : Exa m i n in g a

 L in g u is ti c M a cro ja m i~ .   Colin Renfrew and 

David Nettle, (ed .). pp.93-112.

Cambridge:   The McDonald Institute for 

Archaeological Research.

Greenberg,Joseph 1996.   In Defense of 

Amerind.   In tern~onalJourna l   0/ Am en 'c a n L in g u is ti c s.   62(2):  131-64.

--- 1987.   L a n g u a g e i n t h e A m en ' cas .   Stanford:

Stanford University Press.

Greenberg,   Joseph, Christy Turner, and 

Stephen Zegura 1986. The Settlement of 

the Americas: A Comparison of the

Linguistic, Dental, and Genetic Evidence.

C u m n t A n t h ro p o lo g y .   27(5):477-497.

Guy,]'   B.  M. 1980.   Expen 'men ta lGlo t tochronolo l !J: Bas ic Methods and Resu l t s .

Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.

Haas, Mary 1978.   L anguage, CuIJure , and  

 H is to ry .   Stanford: Stanford University

Press.

Schrryer- Proto-Human Language 60

 L a ng u ag e H is to ry , L a ng ua g e C ha ng e , a n d 

 L a ng u ag e Re la ti on sh ip : An In tr o d ua io n to

 H is to n ' ca l and Compara t i ve L ingu i s t i cs .   Berlin:

Mouton de Gruyter.

Krauss, M. 1973. "Na Dene." In   C u m n t  

Trends in L ingu i st ics .   T.   A.   Sebeok ,   (ed.). pp.903-78. Vol.   10. The Hague: Mouton.

Lamb, Sydney M. 1987.   Some proposals for 

linguistic taxonomy.   An thropolog ica l

u'nguist ics.   1(2): 33-49.

Lieberman, P. 1984.   The Biolol!J and Evolution

0/ L a ng u ag e .   Cambridge: Harvard 

University Press.

McMahon,   April, Marissa Lohr, and Robert

McMahon 1999. "Family Trees and Favorite Daughters." In   Nostratic :

Ex a m i n in g a L : n g u is ti c Ma cro f a m if y.   Colin

Renfrew and David Nettle, (ed.). pp.269-

85.   Cambridge: The McDonald Institute

for Archaeological Research.

Moravcisk ,   Edith 1978.   "Language Contact."

In:   Un i v e rs a ls o / H u m a n L a n g u a g e.   Joseph

Greenberg,   (ed.). pp.93-122. California:

Stanford University Press.

 Nettle, David 1999.   "Towards a futurehistory of macro family research." In:

 N ost ra ti c : Exa m t ' n t 'ng a L ingu i s ti c M a crojamify .

Colin Renfrew and Daniel Nettle, (ed .).

 ppA03-19. Cambridge: The McDonald 

Institute for Archaeological Research.

Renfrew, Colin 1998. "Introduction:   The

 Nostratic hypothesis, linguistic

macrofamilies, and prehistoric studies." In:

The Nos tra t i c Mo cro fami fy and L ingu is t ic

Palaeontolol!J. A.  Dolgopolsky, (ed.). pp.vii-

xxii. Cambridge: The McDonald Institutefor Archaeological Research.

--- 1992.   Archaeology, Genetics, and 

Linguistic Diversity.   M a n .   27:445-78.

--- 1990. "Archaeology and Linguistics:   Some

Preliminary Issues." In   W h e n W o r l d s

Col l ide: Indo-Europeans and Pre-Indo-

Totem: The University of Western Ontario Journal of Anthropology, Vol. 10 [2002], Iss. 1, Art. 9

http://ir.lib.uwo.ca/totem/vol10/iss1/9

Page 10: Proto Human Language

8/9/2019 Proto Human Language

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/proto-human-language 10/10

Europeans .   Colin Renfrew, (ed.). pp. 15-24.

Michigan:   Karoma Publishers, Inc.

Ruhlen, Merritt 1994a.   On t h e Or i g i n s o f  

 L angu ages: S tu d ie s in L in gu is ti c Ta xo nom y .

California:   Stanford University Press.

--- 1994b.   T h e O n ' g in o f Language: Tracing the

Ev o lu t io n o f t h e Mo t he r T o n g ue .   New York:

John Wiley&Sons, Inc.

Trask, R . L.   1999. "Why should a language

have any relatives?" In:   Nos tra t ic: Examin ing

a L ingu is t ic Macro fami !J.   Colin Renfrew and 

David Nettle, (ed.). pp.157-76.

Cambridge:   The McDonald Institute for 

Archaeological Research.

Whitney, William D.   1867.   L a n g ua g e a n d t h e

Stu4Y o f Language.   New York.

Schreyer: A Proto-Human Language: Fact or Fiction

Produced by The Berkeley Electronic Press, 2002