Maste Kasa

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/12/2019 Maste Kasa

    1/12

    Marriage and Psychological Well-Being: Some Evidence on Selection into Marriage

    Author(s): Arne MastekaasaSource: Journal of Marriage and Family, Vol. 54, No. 4 (Nov., 1992), pp. 901-911Published by: National Council on Family RelationsStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/353171.

    Accessed: 22/04/2011 23:14

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at.http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless

    you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you

    may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

    Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at .http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ncfr..

    Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed

    page of such transmission.

    JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of

    content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms

    of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    National Council on Family Relationsis collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to

    Journal of Marriage and Family.

    http://www.jstor.org

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ncfrhttp://www.jstor.org/stable/353171?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ncfrhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ncfrhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/353171?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ncfr
  • 8/12/2019 Maste Kasa

    2/12

    ARNE

    MASTEKAASA

    University f

    Oslo

    Marriage

    and

    Psychological Well-Being:

    Some Evidence

    on

    Selection

    Into

    Marriage

    Higherpsychological

    well-beingamong

    married

    as opposedto unmarried ersonsmay be due to

    social selection

    into

    marriage,

    or to

    marriage f-

    fects

    (social

    causation).

    From the selection

    hy-

    pothesis

    it

    follows

    that

    well-being

    at one time

    point

    be

    positively

    related

    o

    the

    subsequent rob-

    ability

    of marrying.

    Using

    ransition

    atemethods

    (Cox regression)

    n a

    sample

    of

    9,

    000

    unmarried

    persons,

    strong

    and

    significant

    relationships

    re

    found.

    The

    predictivepower

    of

    the

    well-being

    measures remains stable

    throughout

    the

    2- to

    4-year

    period of

    observation.

    t

    is concluded hat

    selection

    mayplay

    an

    important art

    in

    producing

    the

    oft-observed

    association between

    marital

    status

    and

    well-being.

    One

    of

    the more well

    established

    indings

    n

    re-

    search

    on

    mental health

    is the

    relationship

    be-

    tween marital

    tatus

    on

    the

    one hand and various

    measures

    of

    psychologicalwell-being

    and

    psychi-

    atric

    symptoms

    n

    the

    other.

    Typically

    t

    has been

    found

    that

    the

    currently

    married

    njoy

    the

    most

    favorable

    position,

    the

    divorcedand widowed

    are

    generallyworstoff, withthe nevermarriedn an

    intermediate

    osition.

    The most

    impressive

    eature

    of

    this

    relation-

    ship

    s its

    consistency

    cross

    ime,

    space,

    and

    mea-

    surement

    procedures.

    Similar results

    have been

    obtained

    with

    attitudinal

    well-being

    measures

    (Blom

    &

    Listhaug,

    1988;

    Clemente&

    Sauer,

    1976;

    Mastekaasa&

    Moum,

    1984;

    Robinson

    &

    Shaver,

    1973), symptom inventories (Bradburn, 1969;

    Pearlin

    &

    Johnson, 1977;

    Srole,

    Langner,

    Michael,

    Opler,

    &

    Rennie,

    1962),

    mental

    hospital-

    ization rates

    (Gove,

    1972a; Martin, 1976;

    Mastekaasa,

    1984;

    Odegaard,1971),

    and suicide

    rates

    (Gove,

    1972b;Mastekaasa,

    1984).

    Some

    re-

    cent results

    may

    indicate hat these differences

    n

    subjective

    well-being

    have

    been

    declining

    n

    recent

    years,

    at least

    in

    the United

    States

    (Glenn

    &

    Weaver,

    1988;

    Lee,

    Seecombe,

    &

    Shehan,

    1991).

    However,

    data

    from

    two

    European

    countriesdo

    not

    show

    the

    same trend

    (Veenhoven, 1984;

    Mastekaasa,

    1984,

    1988).

    Whatever

    he most recent

    trends

    in the rela-

    tionship

    between

    marital tatusand

    psychological

    well-being,

    he

    problem

    of how to

    interpret

    t in

    causal

    terms remains

    unsolved.

    Basically,

    one

    may

    argue

    hat

    (a)

    marital

    tatus

    has

    some effect

    on

    well-being; (b) well-being

    has

    an effect

    on

    marital

    tatus;

    or

    (c)

    both

    well-being

    and

    marital

    status are

    influenced

    by

    one

    or more common

    causes.

    In the literature

    a)

    is often referred

    o

    as a

    social

    causation

    hypothesis,

    where

    b)

    and

    (c)

    may

    be referred o as socialselectionhypotheses.In-

    tuitively,

    both

    types

    of

    hypotheses

    eem

    reason-

    able,

    and

    they

    are

    not

    mutually

    xclusive.

    t

    is not

    difficult

    to

    imagine

    that a

    spouse may provide

    practical,

    emotional,

    and other

    support

    in

    the

    presence

    of

    stresses

    and

    strains,

    support

    that

    is

    not

    as

    readily

    available

    or

    those

    who

    are

    not

    mar-

    ried

    (or

    live as

    married).

    t is

    also

    quite ikely

    hat

    happy

    and

    cheerful

    people

    are

    regarded

    as

    more

    attractive

    marriagepartners

    han

    brooding

    and

    epartment

    of

    Sociology, University

    of

    Oslo,

    P.O. Box 1096

    Blindern,

    N-317

    Oslo, Norway.

    Journal

    of

    Marriage

    nd the

    Family

    54

    (November1992):

    901-911

    901

  • 8/12/2019 Maste Kasa

    3/12

    902

    Journal

    of

    Marriage

    nd the

    Family

    depressive

    ones. Thus the observed

    relationship

    between marital status

    and

    psychological

    well-

    being

    could

    well be

    the

    joint

    result

    of social selec-

    tion and causationprocesses.

    Most

    sociological

    discussions

    of the social

    causation

    and social selection

    hypotheses

    never-

    theless

    conclude hat socialselection

    processes

    re

    of little

    or no

    importance

    n

    explaining

    he associ-

    ation

    between marital

    status and

    psychological

    well-being(Menaghan,

    1985;

    Glenn

    &

    Weaver,

    1988;

    Gove,

    Hughes

    &

    Style,

    1983),

    although

    selectioncould

    be

    more

    important

    n

    connection

    with

    serious

    mental

    disorders

    (cf.

    Pearlin

    &

    Johnson,

    1977).

    The

    empirical

    vidence,

    however,

    is

    generally

    of

    a

    highly

    indirect

    kind,

    and there-

    fore alsoquite nconclusive. nparticular,mostof

    the relevant tudiesuse

    only

    cross-sectional

    ata,

    which

    makes

    causal

    nferencesdifficult.

    If

    the

    association

    between

    subjective

    well-

    being

    and

    marital

    status

    is at least

    in

    part

    the

    result

    of

    a selection

    process,

    we

    should

    expect

    measures

    of

    subjective

    well-being

    aken

    at

    one

    time to

    predict

    the

    probability of

    changes

    in

    marital

    tatus

    at

    later ime

    points;

    people

    with ow

    levels

    of

    well-being

    hould

    be

    more

    likely

    to

    re-

    main unmarried

    r to divorce

    f

    already

    married.

    In this studyI test the hypothesisas it applies

    to

    the never married.More

    specifically,

    using

    a

    sample

    of

    about

    9,000

    unmarried

    men and

    women,

    I

    examine

    whether

    measures

    f

    well-being

    made at

    one

    time

    point

    predict

    transition

    nto

    marriage

    n

    the

    following

    months

    and

    years.

    A

    statistical

    elationship

    etween

    psychological

    well-

    being

    and

    the

    subsequent robability

    f

    marrying

    provides

    upport

    or

    the selection

    hypothesis,

    but

    does

    not

    imply

    rejection

    of

    the

    social

    causation

    hypothesis.

    As

    noted

    above,

    social selectionand

    social

    causation

    may

    both contribute

    o

    the dif-

    ference in well-beingbetween marriedand un-

    married

    people.

    PREVIOUS

    ESEARCH

    As far

    as the

    present

    author

    s

    aware,

    ongitudinal

    studies

    of

    the

    relationship

    between

    subjective

    well-being

    or

    mental

    health)

    and the

    probability

    of

    transition

    nto

    marriage

    ave

    not

    been

    reported

    in

    the

    literature.

    However,

    Menaghan

    (1985)

    analyzed

    he

    relationship

    between

    depressive

    af-

    fect

    and

    subsequent

    divorce.

    Her

    sample

    con-

    sistedof 790married

    eople

    whowere nterviewed

    in

    1972

    and

    again

    in

    1976.

    Using ordinary

    east

    squares,

    marital tatus

    n

    1976

    (whether

    divorced

    or

    still

    married)

    was

    regressed

    n 1972

    measures

    of

    depression,

    marital

    distress,

    and other

    relevant

    variables.No significant ffect of depressionwas

    found.

    However,

    he

    number

    of

    people divorcing

    in

    the

    4

    years

    between1972

    and 1976was

    only

    32

    (or

    4%).

    Any analysis

    based

    on

    a

    comparison

    of

    these

    32

    persons

    with those who did

    not divorce

    s

    therefore

    highly susceptible

    o

    random

    sampling

    and

    measurement rrors.

    Moreover,

    use of linear

    regression

    r

    discriminant

    nalysis

    s

    problematic

    when the

    dependent

    variable is

    so

    extremely

    skewed as

    is the case

    in

    this

    study

    (Goodman,

    1976).

    As

    noted

    above,

    most

    empirical

    tudies

    on

    the

    socialcausation-selectionssuearebasedon cross-

    sectionaldata.

    Gove et al.

    (1983)

    argue

    that

    if

    a

    social selection

    process

    s

    operating,

    he

    associa-

    tion

    between marital status and

    subjective

    well-

    being

    should be

    substantially

    educed

    f

    one

    con-

    trols

    or

    problems

    an individual

    xperienced

    s

    a

    child.

    The conclusiondrawn

    by

    Gove et al.

    (1983,

    p. 125)

    was

    that the

    relationship between

    hild-

    hood

    problems

    and adult

    mental

    health]

    is

    quite

    weak and

    not

    strong

    enough

    to

    alterthe relation-

    ship

    between

    present

    marital status and

    mental

    healthsubstantially. At best, this can be inter-

    preted

    as evidence

    against

    social selectioneffects

    due

    to

    childhood

    experiences;

    he

    possibility

    of

    selectiondue

    to

    genetic

    factors remains.

    Equally

    important,

    he

    validity

    of

    retrospective

    measures

    of

    childhood

    experiences

    s

    obviously

    doubtful.

    A third

    approach

    o

    the social causation-selec-

    tion

    issue

    is

    to

    compare

    differences

    between

    mar-

    ried and

    unmarried

    ersons

    across

    age categories.

    If

    a

    selection

    process

    is

    operating (and

    in

    the

    absence

    of

    complicating

    ohort

    effects)

    one

    would

    expect

    he association

    between

    marriage

    nd well-

    beingto increasewithage,as thehappiestandthe

    most

    marriageable

    eople

    are

    gradually

    elected

    out

    of

    the unmarried

    ategory.

    Littleevidence

    or

    selectionhas been

    found n studies

    employing

    his

    approach

    (Glenn

    &

    Weaver, 1988; Mastekaasa,

    1988).

    Glenn

    and

    Weaver

    also carriedout

    cohort

    analyses

    based

    on the

    same

    type

    of

    reasoning;

    n

    these

    analyses,

    oo,

    no

    evidence

    or

    selectionwas

    found.

    Needless

    to

    say,

    inferencesbased

    on

    this

    type

    of

    analysis

    are

    highly

    indirect and rest

    on

    several

    trong

    and dubious

    assumptions.

    Someresearchers

    ave

    argued

    hatthe

    subjec-

    tive

    well-being

    f

    widows

    and

    widowers

    s of

    par-

    ticular interest

    if one

    wants

    to

    disentangle

    he

  • 8/12/2019 Maste Kasa

    4/12

    Marriage

    and

    Psychological Well-Being

    903

    underlying

    ausal

    processes.

    The

    argument

    s

    that

    one

    may

    assume a

    priori

    that selection

    nto

    the

    widow(er) ategory

    s

    not

    significantly

    nfluenced

    by social selection(Blom& Listhaug,1988).Ac-

    cordingly,

    widowed

    persons

    should

    not

    differ

    from

    married

    nes,

    controlling

    or

    age

    and other

    relevant

    variables.Durkheim's

    1970) analysis

    of

    suicide

    provides

    an

    argumentalong

    these

    lines.

    However,

    n

    modern western

    societies

    there

    is a

    considerable umber

    of

    widowed

    persons

    who

    re-

    marry; although

    there

    is

    no

    selection

    into

    the

    population

    of

    widowed

    persons,

    here

    may

    be

    dif-

    ferentialselection

    of

    happy

    and

    unhappypeople

    out

    of

    it.

    To

    sum

    up,

    the

    empirical

    tudiesare

    not

    very

    conclusive.In

    particular,

    ittle or no

    evidence s

    available

    n

    possible

    election ffects

    n

    the transi-

    tion

    from

    unmarried

    o

    married.

    SAMPLE AND VARIABLES

    The

    data used

    in

    this

    paper

    were

    collected

    n

    con-

    nection with a

    medical

    screening

    of

    the entire

    population

    of

    one

    of

    Norway's

    19 counties. The

    county

    of

    Nord-Tr6ndelag

    s

    comparatively

    ural

    and

    sparsely

    populated

    (6

    people

    per square

    kilometer),witha totalpopulationof 127,000 in-

    cluding

    children).

    During

    the

    period

    January

    1984

    to

    February

    1986

    all

    adults

    aged

    20 and

    above

    were

    screened

    for

    hypertension,

    diabetes,

    and

    lung

    disease. On

    arrival

    at

    the

    screening, participants

    were

    sup-

    posed

    to

    hand

    in

    a

    one-page

    self-administered

    questionnaire

    which

    had

    been distributed

    y

    mail

    as a

    part

    of

    the

    invitation

    to

    participate

    n

    the

    screening

    Questionnaire ).

    A

    second

    question-

    naire

    (QuestionnaireI)

    was handed

    o

    all

    partici-

    pants

    as

    they

    left

    the

    screening,

    with

    the

    instruc-

    tion to fill inthisquestionnaires soon aspossible

    and return

    t

    by

    mail to

    the

    investigators.

    The

    subpopulation

    of

    interest

    to

    us is never

    married

    persons

    n

    the

    20-39

    age range,

    all

    in

    all

    13,276

    people.

    I

    exclude

    n

    most

    analysespersons

    reporting

    hat

    they

    lived

    together

    with

    a

    person

    of

    the

    opposite sex,

    as well

    as those

    living per-

    manently

    n

    institutions.This

    brings

    the size

    of

    our

    subpopulation

    down

    to

    9,683;

    3,252

    women

    and

    6,431

    men.

    Generally

    peakingparticipation

    nd

    response

    rates in

    the Nord-Trdndelag tudy were very

    satisfactory;

    8%0

    anded

    n

    Questionnaire

    ,

    and

    75%0

    eturned

    Questionnaire

    I.

    For our

    subpopu-

    lation

    of

    relatively

    young

    and never

    married

    respondents,

    however,

    the

    results were

    not

    as

    good.

    Overall

    usable

    Questionnaire

    data were

    obtained rom2,089womenand4,125 men,yield-

    ing fairlyacceptable

    esponse

    rates

    of

    64.3%

    and

    64.1%,

    respectively.

    As

    far as the

    Questionnaire

    II

    dataare

    concerned,however,

    he

    response

    ates

    are less than

    satisfactory;

    0.20%o

    or

    women

    and

    43.6% for

    men.

    Questionnaire

    contained

    only

    a

    single

    well-

    being item,

    whereas everal

    additonal tems

    were

    included

    in

    Questionnaire

    I.

    Due to the

    low

    response

    ate

    for

    Questionnaire

    I,

    we

    shall

    never-

    theless

    have

    o

    rely

    o a

    considerable xtent

    on

    the

    single

    Questionnaire

    item. We

    may

    use the

    some-

    whatricher

    Questionnaire

    Idatato

    get

    some dea

    of how

    vulnerablehe

    resultsare

    to

    the

    reliance

    n

    the

    single-item

    measure.

    The

    questionnaire

    data

    provide

    information

    about marital

    status,

    psychological

    well-being,

    and other variablesat

    one

    specific

    ime

    point.

    By

    making

    use

    of

    public marriageregisters

    we

    are

    able to addinformationabout

    changes

    n

    marital

    status

    in

    subsequent

    years,

    more

    specifically

    or

    the

    years

    1984

    to

    1987.

    Since

    the

    screening

    tself

    was

    carried ut

    during

    he

    period

    January

    1984 o

    February 1986, we have data on subsequent

    changes

    n

    marital

    tatus

    for

    a

    period

    varying

    be-

    tween 22

    and

    47

    months,

    depending

    on

    when

    a

    particular

    ndividual

    was screened.

    Using

    this

    information,

    we

    define our

    depen-

    dent

    variable

    MARRIAGE

    o

    take

    the

    value 1 if

    an individual

    marries

    during

    he

    time

    of

    observa-

    tion,

    and

    to

    be 0

    otherwise.The

    TIME

    between

    the

    start of the

    period

    of

    observation,

    hat

    is,

    the

    screening

    and the

    collection

    of

    questionnaire

    data,

    and

    marriage

    s

    measured

    n

    months.

    For

    those who

    do

    not

    marry

    during

    the

    period

    of

    observation, hat is, for so-calledcensoredobser-

    vations,

    TIME

    is

    defined as the

    length

    of

    the

    observational

    eriod.

    The

    independent

    ariable

    f

    primary

    nterest s

    psychological

    well-being.

    As noted

    above,

    Ques-

    tionnaire

    I

    contained

    a

    single

    well-being

    tem,

    more

    specifically

    a

    standard

    7-point

    overall

    life

    satisfaction tem.

    The

    response

    ategories

    were

    all

    labeled,

    ranging

    from

    very

    satisfied

    to

    very

    dissatisfied.

    We refer to

    this

    one-item

    measure

    as SATISF.

    QuestionnaireI containedan identicalsatis-

    faction

    measure,

    but

    also some

    additonalwell-

    being

    items

    allowing

    us to

    constructa

    four-item

  • 8/12/2019 Maste Kasa

    5/12

    904

    Journal

    of

    Marriage

    and the

    Family

    additive

    ndex

    (WELLB).

    In

    addition

    o

    the two

    identical atisfaction

    measures,

    he index ncludes

    two

    semanticdifferential ike items

    measured

    on

    7-point

    scales:

    verystrong

    and

    fit-very

    tired

    and

    rundown,

    and

    very depressed-very

    happy.

    The

    index

    score is the arithmetic

    average

    of

    the

    responses

    on

    the

    four items

    (if

    a

    respondent

    did

    not

    answerone

    or two of

    the

    items,

    she is

    given

    her

    average

    core

    on

    the

    remaining

    tems).

    The correlations

    among

    the

    four

    well-being

    itemsare

    n

    the

    .48

    to

    .70

    range,

    and

    the

    reliability

    (Cronbach'sa)

    of the

    summated

    scores is

    .82.

    Factor

    analysis

    (principal

    axes

    factoring)

    of

    the

    items

    provide

    clear

    evidence

    of

    unidimensionality

    (an

    eigenvalue

    f

    2.6

    for

    the first factorand

    .6

    for

    thesecond).If theQuestionnaireand II satisfac-

    tion

    items are assumed

    o

    be

    equally

    reliable,

    he

    test-retest orrelation

    of

    .70

    between

    these items

    provides

    an estimate

    of

    the

    reliability

    f

    the

    single

    item SATISF measure.The internal

    consistency

    and test-retestestimatesare

    in

    line with

    results

    reported

    n

    the

    quality

    of

    life literature

    e.g.,

    Ab-

    bey

    &

    Andrews, 1985;

    Diener,

    1984;

    Larsen &

    Diener,

    1985). Although

    these results

    must

    be

    considered

    uitesatisfactory y

    most

    standards,

    t

    should

    be

    noted that less

    than

    perfect reliability

    can be

    expected

    to

    bias coefficients downward

    andto increase tandard rrors,

    herebymaking

    t

    more

    difficult

    to

    obtain

    evidence

    of

    selectionef-

    fects.

    In

    addition

    o

    the

    well-being

    measures,

    we

    in-

    clude

    some

    additional

    ndependent

    variables

    as

    controls.

    The selection of

    control variables is

    based

    on

    previous

    research

    on

    transitions

    nto

    marriage,

    ubject,

    of

    course,

    to

    the limitations

    f

    the

    current

    data

    set

    (Elder

    &

    Rockwell,

    1976;

    Hogan,

    1978;

    Waite&

    Spitze, 1981).

    First

    of

    all,

    we

    may

    note that

    all

    analyses

    are

    carried ut separatelyormenand women.Ageis

    also

    partly

    aken

    into

    consideration

    y

    means

    of

    subgroupanalyses,

    but

    in

    additionmost

    analyses

    include he

    variable

    AGE,

    measured

    n

    years.

    The

    probability

    f

    marriage

    an

    be

    expected

    o

    differ

    between

    sparsely

    and more

    denselypopu-

    lated

    areas.

    In

    particular,

    elective

    migration

    of

    young

    womento

    the

    cities

    may

    create mbalances

    in

    the

    marriage

    market.

    Using

    a

    standard for

    classification

    evelopedby

    the

    Norwegian

    Central

    Bureau of

    Statistics,

    the

    variable CENTRAL

    distinguishes

    etween

    relatively

    central

    or

    urban

    (value 1) and relatively peripheral (value 0)

    municipalities.

    Previous research

    has

    demonstrated hat one

    of the most

    importantpredictors

    of

    marriage

    s

    the extent to

    which

    a

    person

    is

    engaged

    n

    ac-

    tivities that

    consume

    periods

    of

    time

    during

    the

    transition

    to

    adulthood,

    such as an

    additional

    year

    of

    schooling,

    service

    n

    the

    military,

    and

    im-

    migration

    rom

    abroad

    (Hogan, 1978,

    p.

    169).

    Using

    the

    gainfully

    employed

    as a

    reference

    ate-

    gory,

    we

    include

    dummy

    variables or

    education/

    military

    service

    (EDUCMIL)

    and

    not

    working

    (NOT

    WORK).

    We use

    two health

    measures. LL

    takes

    on

    the

    value

    1 if

    the

    respondent

    eports

    hat she

    or

    he

    has

    any

    diseasewhichrestricts

    daily

    activities

    n

    some

    way,

    and 0

    otherwise.

    SUBH

    is

    a

    measure

    f

    self-

    assessedhealth,withvalue1for poor and not

    quite good

    healthand 0

    otherwise.

    We do not

    include

    measures

    of

    occupational

    and educational

    attainmentas

    independent

    ari-

    ables. For

    the

    relatively

    young sample

    analyzed

    here,

    use

    of

    these variables s

    fairlyproblematic.

    Moreover,

    previous

    esearchhas shown

    hat

    these

    variableshave

    very

    weak

    relationships

    with

    sub-

    jective well-being.

    Omitting

    these

    variables is

    therefore

    not

    likely

    o

    biasthe

    estimated

    ffects

    of

    well-being

    on

    the

    probability

    f

    marrying.

    Descriptive

    tatistics

    or

    the

    independent

    ari-

    ables are

    given

    in Table 1.

    TABLE

    .

    DESCRIPTIVETATISTICS

    Women Men

    Values

    M

    SD

    M

    SD

    CENTRAL

    0-1

    .749 .434 .700

    .458

    AGE

    20-39

    26.172 4.875

    26.769 5.007

    SUBH

    0-1 .087 .281 .079

    .269

    ILL

    0-1

    .126

    .332 .165

    .371

    NOTWORK

    0-1 .308

    .462

    .137

    .343

    EDUCMIL

    0-1

    .234 .423

    .145

    .352

    SATISF

    1-7

    5.623 1.060 5.528 1.128

    WELLB 1-7 5.307 .902 5.353 .873

    MARRIAGE 0-1 .113 .317 .077 .267

    TIME

    1-47

    35.258 8.017

    35.257 7.504

    STATISTICAL

    ETHODSND

    MODEL

    PECIFICATION

    The

    question

    asked

    in

    this

    study

    is

    whether

    sub-

    jective

    well-being

    affects the

    probability

    of

    marry-

    ing.

    Since

    most

    individuals do not

    marry

    during

    the

    period

    of

    observation,

    most

    of

    the

    observa-

    tions are censored.

    Furthermore,

    he

    length

    of

    the

    observational

    period

    varies

    (from

    22

    to 47

    months).The researchquestion s thereforebest

    answered

    by

    applying

    methods

    for

    the

    analysis

    of

  • 8/12/2019 Maste Kasa

    6/12

    Marriage

    nd

    Psychological

    Well-Being

    905

    event

    history

    or

    durationdata.

    More

    specifically,

    we

    may

    use

    so-called Cox

    regression,

    or

    semi-

    parametric

    roportional

    hazards

    stimation.

    In this method,the rateof transition theso-

    called hazard

    rate)

    from

    the

    unmarried o

    the

    married

    tate can

    be

    expressed

    as

    (cf.

    Blossfeld,

    Hamerle,

    &

    Mayer, 1989):

    7r(t

    x)

    =

    ro(t)exp(x'O)

    (1)

    or,

    equivalently,

    ln[7r(t

    x)]

    =

    ln[ro(t)]

    +

    x'3

    (2)

    In

    these

    equations,

    t

    is

    time

    since

    the

    screening

    and

    x

    is a

    vector

    of

    independent

    ariables;

    r0

    nd

    0

    are

    parameters.

    The

    logarithmically

    rans-

    formed transition rate for an individual is

    specified

    as

    the sum of two

    terms.

    The

    first

    term,

    ln[or(t)],

    allows

    the

    transitionrate

    to

    vary

    over

    the time of

    observation,

    but the exact

    functional

    form of

    this time

    dependence

    does

    not

    have

    to

    be

    specified since

    so-called

    partial

    ikelihood stima-

    tion

    is

    used).

    The

    second

    erm,

    x'P,

    allows

    he

    log

    of

    the transition ate to

    vary

    as a linearfunction

    of the

    independent

    variables

    the

    x

    vector).

    Ex-

    ponentiating

    he estimated

    coefficients,

    they

    can

    be

    interpreted

    s follows:

    For

    each

    unit

    ncrease

    n

    an independentvariable, the transition rate is

    multiplied by

    its

    exponentiated

    coefficient

    (Allison,

    1984,

    p.

    28).

    Roughly

    speaking

    the

    estimated

    hanges

    n

    the transition

    ate

    can

    be in-

    terpreted

    s

    changes

    n

    the

    probability

    of

    making

    a

    transition. f we

    find,

    for

    instance,

    hat

    the esti-

    mated

    coefficient

    for

    a

    7-point

    satisfactionmea-

    sure

    is

    .20,

    corresponding

    o

    an

    exponentiated

    coefficient

    of

    1.22,

    the

    interpretation

    s that a unit

    increase

    n

    satisfaction s

    associated

    with a

    22%

    increase

    n

    the

    probability

    f

    marrying.

    As noted

    above,

    the

    partial

    ikelihood

    method

    allows us to estimatethe effects of a set of in-

    dependent

    variables

    without

    having

    to

    specify

    how the

    transition ate

    varies

    over

    ime.

    However,

    the method

    s basedon the

    assumption

    f

    propor-

    tional

    likelihoods;

    that

    is,

    the

    proportional

    dif-

    ference

    in

    the transitionrate

    between

    categories

    on

    an

    independent

    variable

    should be

    constant

    over

    time.

    Returning

    o our

    exampleabove,

    a unit

    increase

    n

    satisfaction

    houldbe

    associatedwith

    a

    22% increase

    in

    the

    probability

    of

    marrying

    throughout

    he

    period

    of

    observation.If the

    ef-

    fect of

    satisfactiondeclines

    over

    time,

    the

    propor-

    tionallikelihood

    assumption

    s violated.

    The

    time

    dependence

    of

    the

    effect is

    by

    no

    meansa

    purely

    echnical

    matter,

    however.

    argue

    below

    that a selection

    theory

    of

    the

    relationship

    between

    marriage

    nd

    well-being

    mplies

    hat

    the

    predictive owerof psychologicalwell-beingmea-

    suresdoes not declineover

    time.

    The time

    dependence

    of the

    well-being

    effect

    canbe

    analyzed

    n several

    ways.

    One

    possibility

    s

    to

    expand

    the hazard model

    by including

    a

    specific

    time

    dependence arameter

    moreor

    less

    like an

    interactioneffect

    in

    an

    ordinary

    inear

    regression

    model.

    In some of the

    models

    esti-

    mated

    below,

    the

    effect

    of the

    well-being

    measure

    is

    specified

    as:

    Owellb

    =

    ao0

    +

    CZ[ln(t)

    - 2.4849]

    (3)

    The effect of well-being

    (fwellb)

    is allowed to de-

    pend

    on the

    log

    of the time

    since

    the

    screening

    t)

    and

    c1

    can be

    interpreted

    as the

    change

    in

    the

    well-being

    effect

    corresponding

    o a

    doubling

    of

    the time

    since

    he

    screening;

    a

    gives

    he

    estimated

    effect of

    psychological

    well-being

    fter 12

    months

    (i.e.,

    when

    [ln(t)

    -

    2.4849]

    equals

    0).

    If

    the

    estimateof

    a

    ca

    is not

    significant

    we

    may

    take

    this

    as an

    indication hat the

    effect

    of

    satisfaction

    s

    approximately

    onstant

    over

    the

    period

    of

    obser-

    vation.

    To check the robustnessof the results,I also

    examine he time

    dependence

    ssue

    by

    estimating

    series

    of

    models

    in

    which

    the

    time

    between the

    well-being

    measurement

    and

    the

    start

    of

    the

    period

    of

    observation s

    gradually

    ncreased.In

    the first

    analysis,

    he

    period

    of

    observation

    tarts

    at thetime of the

    well-being

    measurement

    i.e.,

    at

    the time

    of

    the

    screening).

    n

    the

    second

    analysis,

    those who

    marry

    within

    a

    6-month

    period

    ollow-

    ing

    the

    initial

    measurement

    re

    excluded

    rom

    the

    analysis.

    We ask in

    effect: Does

    overall

    life

    satisfaction

    predict

    the

    probability

    of

    marrying,

    given that a person has not marriedwithinthe

    first

    6

    months

    after

    answering

    he

    well-being

    ues-

    tions.

    This

    analysis

    s

    then

    repeated

    by

    excluding

    those who

    marry

    within

    12,

    18,

    and 24

    months

    following

    he

    screening,

    espectively.

    The

    present

    analyses

    may

    give

    an

    indication

    of

    the existenceand

    magnitude

    of

    selection

    effects,

    but

    it

    provides

    very

    little

    information

    about the

    actual

    content

    of

    a

    selection

    process.

    In the

    first

    place,

    we

    have

    data

    on

    individuals

    nly,

    whereas

    marriage

    s the result

    of

    an interaction

    etween

    at

    least)

    two

    persons.

    Neitherdo we

    have

    informa-

    tion

    about

    possible

    relevant

    behavioral dif-

    ferences

    between

    happy

    and

    unhappy people.

  • 8/12/2019 Maste Kasa

    7/12

    906

    Journal

    of Marriage

    and the

    Family

    Simply

    appearing

    o be

    happy

    and

    cheerful

    may

    make

    a

    person

    more

    attractive as

    partner

    or

    spouse.

    But low

    well-beingmay

    also

    be

    correlated

    withcharacteristicsikepassivity,ntroversion, r

    low

    interpersonal

    competence

    (Diener,

    1984;

    Costa &

    McCrae,

    1980;

    cf. also

    Hansson, Jones,

    &

    Carpenter,

    1984).

    Moreover,

    happy

    persons

    may

    be

    more

    optimistic

    about

    the

    future,

    and

    thereforemore

    ready

    o run the

    risk of a marital

    relationship.

    Although

    the

    data

    do not

    allow a more de-

    tailed

    investigation

    f the

    hypothesized

    election

    processes,

    he

    above

    remarks t

    least serve

    o

    sen-

    sitize us

    to

    the

    possibility

    hat

    selection

    processes

    may

    be

    different

    for

    men

    and

    women,

    and for

    persons

    at different

    age

    levels. The directionof

    potential

    differences

    re,

    however,

    not

    easily

    pre-

    dicted.

    To the

    extent

    that traditional ex roles are

    still

    important,

    personal

    characteristics

    like

    passivity

    or

    introversion

    may

    be

    particularly

    damaging

    o

    men,

    whereas

    generally ppearing

    o

    be

    happy

    and

    cheerful

    may

    be more

    mportant

    or

    women.

    Another

    reason for

    expecting

    different

    selec-

    tion

    processes

    or

    men and

    women

    has

    been

    sug-

    gested

    by

    Bernard

    1972).

    She

    argues

    that men

    tend to prefermarriagewithsociallyand intellec-

    tually

    inferior

    women.

    Characteristics

    ike in-

    telligence

    and

    independence

    hus

    may

    reduce a

    woman's

    probability

    f

    finding

    a

    suitable

    partner,

    whereas

    they

    may

    have the

    opposite

    effect

    for

    men.

    Selection

    processes

    may

    also

    differ between

    people

    at

    different

    age

    levels. The

    personality

    nd

    behavioral

    haracteristicshat

    makea

    20-year-old

    man an

    attractive

    marriage

    artner

    to perhaps

    n

    18-year-old

    woman) may

    be

    different from the

    characteristics

    hat

    are most

    important

    for

    a

    35-year-oldman. Inaddition, hemarriageate t-

    self

    depends

    of

    course

    very

    muchon

    age, typically

    following

    an

    inverted

    U-shape.

    In

    Norway,

    the

    probability

    of

    marriage

    s

    greatest

    or

    persons

    n

    their

    mid-20s.

    To

    allow for

    differences

    n

    the

    selection

    proc-

    ess,

    the

    models

    are

    estimated

    eparately

    or

    men

    and

    women

    and

    (with

    the

    exception

    noted

    below)

    for two

    different

    age

    categories

    of

    approximately

    equal

    size

    (20-25

    and

    26-39

    years

    of

    age).

    Within

    each

    of

    these

    categories

    heeffects of

    the

    indepen-

    dentvariables

    re not assumed o

    depend

    on

    age.

    The

    effect of

    age

    on the

    logarithm

    of the

    transi-

    tion rate is

    assumed o

    be linear.

    We

    expect

    the

    direct

    effect of

    age

    to be

    positive

    n the

    20-25

    age

    intervaland

    negative

    n the

    26-39 interval.

    To

    obtain more

    stable estimates

    n the more

    detailedanalysesof the time dependenceof the

    well-being

    ffect,

    the

    sample

    s

    not subdivided

    y

    age.

    However,

    the

    analyses

    are still carried

    out

    separately

    or men and women.As a second

    best

    solution

    to the

    problem

    of

    age

    effects,

    we use a

    so-called stratified

    analysis. Age

    is

    grouped

    nto

    four strata

    20-24,

    25-29,

    30-34,

    and

    35-39),

    and

    the

    marriage

    ate as well as the time

    dependence

    of this rate is

    allowed

    to

    vary

    between he strata

    (cf.

    Blossfeld,

    et al.

    1989,

    p.

    149ff).

    THEPREDICTIVEOWERF

    THE

    WELL-BEING

    EASURES

    Table

    2

    contains

    results

    rom

    separateregression

    analyses

    of

    four

    subsamples,

    women divided

    nto

    the 20-25 and 26-39

    age

    categories,

    and men

    divided

    into

    the same

    categories.

    The

    estimated

    effect

    of

    overall ife satisfaction

    s

    positive

    n

    all

    four

    subsamples

    nd

    clearlysignificant

    n two

    of

    them. The

    clearest

    evidence

    or

    a

    selection

    effect

    is

    found

    for

    women

    n

    the 20-25

    age

    category

    and

    for

    men 26-39

    years

    of

    age.

    Using

    a .05

    signifi-

    cance evel(anda one-tailed est, which s appro-

    priatehere),

    he effect

    of

    satisfaction

    n the rate

    of transition nto

    marriage

    s

    significant

    or

    women

    n the

    26-39

    agecategory,

    oo.

    In the model

    used

    here,

    the

    effect

    of

    satisfac-

    tion

    is

    allowed o declineoverthe

    period

    of obser-

    vation.

    The

    TIME

    variable

    s

    scaled so

    that

    the

    estimated coefficient

    for

    SATISF

    can

    be inter-

    preted

    as the

    effect

    of

    satisfaction

    when

    TIME

    s

    equal

    to

    12

    months.

    There s

    no

    evidence

    hat the

    effect

    of

    satisfaction

    s

    really

    declining

    as

    a

    func-

    tion of time. In none of the four

    regression

    analysess thecoefficient or theinteractionerm,

    SATISF

    x

    In(TIME),

    ignificant;

    and

    it has the

    expected negative

    sign

    in

    only

    two

    of the four

    analyses.

    In view

    of

    these

    results,

    t

    may

    be

    preferable

    o

    assume

    hatthe

    effects

    of

    satisfaction

    reconstant

    over

    time,

    and

    to estimate

    he model without an

    interaction

    term. The estimates

    appearing

    in

    parentheses

    re

    based

    on

    that model.

    (This

    mod-

    ification

    of

    the

    model affects

    the

    estimated

    oeffi-

    cients for the

    control

    variables

    very

    little,

    and a

    new set of estimates s thereforenot shown.)

    As one

    would

    expect given

    the small and un-

    stable

    nteraction

    ffects,

    the main

    pattern

    of the

  • 8/12/2019 Maste Kasa

    8/12

    Marriage

    and

    Psychological

    Well-Being

    907

    TABLE 2. Cox

    REGRESSION

    OF MARRIAGE ON OVERALL LIFE SATISFACTION AND CONTROLS

    Women

    Men

    20-25

    Years

    26-39 Years

    20-25 Years 26-39

    Years

    b Sb b bb b Sb b Sb

    CENTRAL

    .1022 .1832

    -.3393

    .2581 -.1423

    .1661 .0264

    .1808

    AGE

    -.0271 .0608 -.1149

    .0380 .1919 .0590 -.1072 .0252

    SUBH

    .0600 .3750

    -.0573

    .4751 .3971 .3560 .4579 .3026

    ILL

    -.2957

    .3191

    .1734 .3722 -.8837 .3077 -.4117 .2735

    NOTWORK

    -.0846 .1986 -.4031 .2827 -.2081 .2473 -.0510 .2740

    EDUCMIL

    -.1360

    .1843 .2081 .3562 .0925 .1843 .0468 .3492

    SATISF

    .3015*** .0935 .2198* .1267 .0235 .0953 .2133** .0829

    (SATISF)a

    (.3118***) (.0826)

    (.1868)

    (.1231) (.0709)

    (.0740)

    (.1923*)

    (.0813)

    SATISF

    x

    In(TIME)

    .0298

    .1067 -.1971 .1500 .0824 .1070 -.1360 .0897

    Log-likelihood

    -114.5241 -476.1625 -1252.4554

    -1067.2962

    n

    1,251

    838

    2,118 2,007

    Proportion marrying

    .130

    .088

    .081 .073

    Response

    ate

    .618

    .684

    .633 .650

    aEstimate based

    on

    the

    assumption

    f constanteffect of satisfaction

    no

    time

    dependence).

    Probability levels for SATISF and SATISF x In(TIME) coefficients (one-tailed t tests):

    *p

    < .05.

    **p