RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    1/24

    RES-000-23-0292 22

    Interviewing to Detect Deception (grant RES-000-23-0292)

    Aldert Vrij

    Samantha Mann

    Ron Fisher

    Becky Milne

    Ray Bull

    Objectives

    We are pleased to report that the project went according to plan. We have carried

    out all four experiments as outlined in the proposal. In the original proposal we refer to

    four experiments, whereby Experiments 1 to 3 consisted of two parts, an interview part

    and a lie detection part. Throughout this report we will refer to these two parts as separate

    experiments, and we will therefore discuss the results of seven experiments.

    Background

    In their police manual, Inbau, Reid, Buckley, and Jayne (2001) 1 present three

    strategies aimed at detecting deceit. These are: (i) the focusing on visual cues strategy;

    (ii) the Baseline Method strategy; and (iii) the Behaviour Analysis Interview strategy. In

    this ESRC project, we examined to which extent those three traditional methods could

    discriminate between truth tellers and liars. However, we were pessimistic about their

    1 What Inbau et al. (2001) have to say about lie detection is important because their manual is influentialand commonly used by police and military interrogators (Gudjonsson, 2003).

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    2/24

    RES-000-23-0292 23

    success, and therefore developed a new strategy to detect deception, called the cognitive

    load approach, which we tested in the present project.

    Traditional Methods to Detect Deceit

    Although in interview settings police detectives could concentrate on both the

    suspects behaviour and speech, police manuals typically focus more on behaviour

    (visual cues) than on speech related cues to deceit. For example, when Inbau et al. (2001)

    discuss the visual (e.g., eye contact, movements), vocal (e.g., response latency, response

    length) and verbal (e.g., evasiveness) cues that they claim are related to deception

    (Chapter 9), the visual cues section is the largest section. This is remarkable because

    deception research has demonstrated that most visual cues, including most of those

    discussed by Inbau et al. in Chapter 9, are not reliable indicators of deceit (DePaulo,

    Lindsay, Malone, Muhlenbruck, Charlton, & Cooper, 2003). In contrast, vocal cues

    (DePaulo et al., 2003) and verbal cues (Masip, Sporer, Garrido, & Herrero, 2005; Vrij,

    2005) are more reliable cues to deception. Concentrating too much on visual cues will

    therefore hamper lie detection due to the lack of diagnostic cues an observer could rely

    upon. Concentrating on visual cues will further encourage the use of stereotypes,

    especially when speech is ignored and only nonverbal cues taken into account, as

    observers have little to go on (Bond & DePaulo, 2006). Those views are related to how

    liars behave (e.g., liars show gaze aversion and fidget) rather than how truth tellers

    behave. As a result, concentrating on the visual cues may result in a lie bias, that is the

    tendency to judge people as liars. We tested in this project the hypothesis that

    concentrating on visual cues will impair lie detection accuracy and will increase a lie bias

    (Experiment 7).

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    3/24

    RES-000-23-0292 24

    The basic assumption behind the Baseline Method strategy (examined in

    Experiments 5 and 6) is that different individuals show different nonverbal and verbal

    responses. This assumption is correct. Some people typically make many movements,

    whilst others do not; some people are very talkative, others are not, some tend to look

    someone else into the eyes when they speak, other do not, etc. (DePaulo & Friedman,

    1998; Vrij, 2000, 2004). In order to control for those individual differences, the Baseline

    Method strategy recommends that police officers examine a suspect's natural, truthful,

    verbal and nonverbal responses during the small-talk part at the beginning of the

    interview, and compare these responses with those shown by the suspect during the actual

    interview (Inbau et al., 2001). Differences in behaviour could then be interpreted as signs

    of deceit.

    This approach is prone to resulting in incorrect judgements because an

    incongruent comparison is made. Not only do different people behave differently in the

    same situation ( inter personal differences), the same person also behaves differently in

    different situations ( intra personal differences) (DePaulo & Friedman, 1998). The

    baseline method as proposed by Inbau et al. (2001) fails to take those intrapersonal

    differences into account. Small-talk and the actual investigation are fundamentally

    different situations. Small-talk conversations are low-stake situations where the suspect's

    responses are unlikely to have any negative consequences. The investigative part of the

    interview, on the other hand, is a high-stake situation where the suspect's responses are

    intensely scrutinised and his/her reactions may easily heighten suspicion. Suspects are

    probably aware of this and therefore, not surprisingly, both guilty and innocent people

    tend to show different behaviours during small-talk compared to the actual interview

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    4/24

    RES-000-23-0292 25

    (Vrij, 1995, 2006). We thus predicted that the baseline method as proposed by Inbau et

    al. would not be effective in discriminating between liars and truth tellers, but were

    hoping that a baseline method that did take the intrapersonal differences into account

    would be useful.

    The Behaviour Analysis Interview (Inbau et al., 2001, examined in Experiments

    1 and 2) is a predetermined standardised list of 15 questions, starting with the question:

    "What is your understanding of the purpose of this interview?", followed by questions

    such as: "Who would have had the best opportunity to commit the crime if they had

    wanted to?" Despite its name, behaviour analysis interview, Inbau et al. (2001) predict

    differences in both nonverbal and verbal responses between liars and truth tellers.

    Regarding the nonverbal responses, Inbau et al. (2001) assume that liars feel less

    comfortable than truth tellers in the police interview situation. As a result, guilty suspects

    are more likely to cross their legs, shift in their chair, look away and perform grooming

    behaviours while answering the BAI questions. Verbal responses should also

    discriminate between truth tellers and liars, because, according to Inbau et al. (2001),

    interviewees' answers will reflect their different attitudes towards the investigation. Liars

    are thought to be less helpful in the interview and do not show the appropriate level of

    concern about being a suspect, whereas truth tellers are more likely to offer helpful

    information, and show an expectancy to be exonerated.

    There is reason to doubt that Inbau's BAI hypothesis will promote effective

    discrimination between truth tellers and liars. The basic premise underlying the nonverbal

    responses, that liars feel less comfortable than truth tellers in a police interview, is not

    universally accepted by the scientific community (National Research Council, 2003). For

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    5/24

    RES-000-23-0292 26

    instance, DePaulo et al. (2003) and Ekman (1985) have argued that in situations where

    the consequences of not being believed are severe, not only liars but also truth tellers will

    be concerned about not being believed.

    Compatible with this view, but refuting Inbau's hypothesis, DePaulo et al.'s

    (2003) meta-analysis of nonverbal correlates of deception demonstrated that liars were no

    more likely than truth tellers to look away, shift in their chair, cross their legs or make

    grooming gestures. In fact, eye contact is not related to deception, and, in direct contrast

    to Inbau et al.'s (2001) assumptions, liars tend to decrease rather than increase their

    movements (DePaulo et al., 2003). The tendency for liars to decrease their movements

    may reflect greater cognitive load borne by liars. Lying is often cognitively more difficult

    then telling the truth (DePaulo et al., 2003; Ekman, 1985), and greater cognitive load

    results in a neglect of body language, reducing overall animation (Ekman, 1985). Liars'

    decreased movements may also be partly attributable to impression management. Liars

    are likely to be even more concerned about the impression they make on others than truth

    tellers (DePaulo et al., 2003; Vrij, 2000; Kassin, 2005; Kassin & Norwick, 2004). Like

    liars, truth tellers are also keen to be seen as truthful. However, truth tellers typically do

    not think that this will require any special effort or attention (DePaulo et al., 2003). In

    other words, truth tellers more often take their credibility for granted (DePaulo et al.,

    2003; Kassin, 2005; Kassin & Norwick, 2004). As a result, liars are more inclined than

    truth tellers to refrain from exhibiting excessive movements that could be construed as

    nervous or suspicious (DePaulo & Kirkendol, 1989).

    This latter impression management explanation (liars put more effort into making

    a convincing impression than truth tellers) also raises doubts about the view that guilty

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    6/24

    RES-000-23-0292 27

    suspects will be less helpful than innocent suspects. This was the premise underlying the

    expected differences in verbal responses to the BAI questions between guilty and

    innocent suspects. Both guilty and innocents suspects will probably believe that being

    helpful will make a positive impression, but perhaps guilty suspects will be even keener

    to make such an impression, because they will take their credibility less for granted.

    The combined cognitive load/impression management hypothesis directly

    opposes Inbau et al. (2001), and hence predicts that (a) liars' answers should be more

    helpful than those of truth tellers and (b) liars' nonverbal responses should appear more

    relaxed than those of truth tellers.

    The Cognitive Load Approach

    Since we had our doubts about the capability of the traditional methods to

    successfully discriminate between liars and truth tellers, we proposed a new lie detection

    approach (Vrij, Fisher, Mann, & Leal, 2006). This approach is based on the premise that,

    in most interviews, lying is cognitively demanding. Several aspects of lying contribute to

    this increased mental load. First, formulating the lie itself may be cognitively taxing.

    Second, as mentioned above, because liars are typically less likely than truth tellers to

    take their credibility for granted liars will be more inclined than truth tellers to monitor

    and control their demeanour in order to appear honest to the lie detector. Monitoring and

    controlling ones own demeanour should be cognitively demanding. Third, because liars

    do not take credibility for granted, they may monitor the interviewer's reactions more

    carefully in order to assess whether they are getting away with their lie (Buller &

    Burgoon, 1996). Carefully monitoring the interviewer also requires cognitive load.

    Fourth, liars may be preoccupied by the task of reminding themselves to act and role-play

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    7/24

    RES-000-23-0292 28

    (DePaulo et al., 2003), which requires extra cognitive effort. Fifth, liars have to suppress

    the truth while they are lying and this is also cognitively demanding (Spence et al., 2004).

    Finally, while activation of the truth often happens automatically, activation of the lie is

    more intentional and deliberate, and thus requires mental effort (Gilbert, 1991; Walczyk,

    Roper, Seemann, & Humphrey, 2003).

    The premise that lying is cognitively demanding has support from a number of

    sources (Vrij, Fisher, Mann, & Leal, 2006). For example, participants in a mock-suspect

    experiment directly assessed their own cognitive load during interviews and reported that

    lying is more cognitively demanding than truth telling (Vrij, Semin, & Bull, 1996); police

    officers who watched videotapes of real life police - suspect interviews thought that the

    suspects were thinking harder when they lied than when they told the truth (Mann & Vrij,

    2006); and deceiving is associated with activating executive 'higher' brain centres such as

    the prefrontal cortex (Spence et al., 2004).

    Lie detectors could exploit the differential levels of cognitive load that liars and

    truth tellers experience by introducing protocols that introduce additional cognitive load.

    Liars, whose cognitive resources will already be depleted by the more demanding act of

    lying, will have fewer cognitive resources left over to satisfactorily address the additional

    cognitive demand, which will result in more substantial differences between liars and

    truth tellers and, subsequently, should facilitate discriminating between liars and truth

    tellers. A method to increase cognitive load is recalling an event in reverse order (Fisher,

    Brennan, & McCauley, 2002).

    We examined the impact of recalling a story in reverse order on lying and lie

    detection in Experiments 3 and 4. Based on our assumptions that (i) lying is cognitively

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    8/24

    RES-000-23-0292 29

    more demanding than truth telling, and (ii) recalling a story in reverse order is cognitively

    more demanding than telling a story in chronological order, we expected liars to show

    more cues to cognitive load than truth tellers, particularly when they were recalling their

    stories in reverse order (Experiment 3). In Experiment 4 we showed police officers a

    selection of those interviews, and examined their ability to discriminate between liars and

    truth tellers in the reverse order and control conditions. Based on the outcome we

    anticipated in Experiment 3, we expected truth and lie detection to be superior in the

    reverse order condition.

    Finally, we examined in what type of interview setting our cognitive load

    approach would be most appropriate (Experiment 1). In their analysis of videotaped

    police interviews with suspects, Moston and Engelberg (1993) observed two different

    styles of starting an interview: Accusatory style and information-gathering style. In the

    accusatory style, the interviewer confronts the suspect with an accusation (e.g., "I think

    you have committed the crime", "Your reactions make me think that you are hiding

    something from me"). In the information-gathering style, the interviewer does not

    confront the suspect with an accusation, but rather asks an open question allowing

    suspects to describe their actions in their own words (e.g., "Describe in as much detail as

    possible what you did between 3pm and 4pm this afternoon?"). We expected

    information-gathering interviews to be more appropriate for our cognitive load approach

    than the accusatory interviews. First, we expected information-gathering interviews to be

    more cognitively demanding than accusatory interviews. In an accusatory interview, the

    respondents' replies are likely to be simple, short denials of the accusations (" I didn't do

    it", "I am innocent", etc.). By comparison, in information-gathering interviews,

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    9/24

    RES-000-23-0292 30

    respondents are encouraged to provide detailed and lengthy statements about their

    activities, which should make their task more cognitively demanding. Second, we

    expected information-gathering interviews to result in more verbal cues to deception than

    accusatory interviews. The more words there are in the suspect's response, the more

    verbal cues there should be to discriminate between liars and truth tellers, simply because

    words are the carriers of verbal cues. As information-gathering interviews will generate

    longer answers, they will contain more verbal cues to deception than will accusatory

    interviews.

    Methods

    In Experiments 1, 3, and 5 participants (undergraduate students) lied or told the

    truth. In Experiments 1 and 3 they lied or told the truth about taking money out of a

    wallet (see the two attached articles for full details of the experimental design); in

    Experiment 5 they lied or told the truth about having prior knowledge about the number

    of dots that appeared on a screen in a count the dots competition. The interviews were

    video recorded and the nonverbal behaviours (gaze, gestures (e.g., illustrators,

    hand/finger movements and self-adaptors), foot/leg movements, eye blinks, latency

    period, pauses, stutters, uhms, speech rate) and speech content (the verbal cues listed in

    the Criteria-Based Content Analysis and Reality Monitoring tools) were coded. In

    Experiment 1 the participants were interviewed in three different ways (accusatory,

    information-gathering, and behaviour analysis interview style) and the impact of the

    interview style on behavioural and verbal responses was examined; in Experiment 3

    participants recalled their stories in either normal or reverse order; and the impact of this

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    10/24

    RES-000-23-0292 31

    instruction on the behavioural and verbal responses were examined; in Experiment 5 the

    behaviour and speech content of liars and truth tellers during the target period (guessing

    the number of dots on a screen while having (liars) or not having (truth tellers) been

    informed the actual number of dots that appeared on the screen) were compared with the

    behaviour and speech content shown by the participants during two control periods,

    whereby one control period showed more similarity with the target period (estimating the

    number of dots on the screen without having prior knowledge) than the other control

    period (small talk at the beginning of the interview). In Experiments 2, 4, and 6,

    observers (police officers) were shown a selection of twelve interviews (six truth tellers

    and six liars) from Experiments 1, 3 and 5 respectively and were asked after each

    interview to judge the veracity of the story-teller. In Experiment 7, observers (police

    officers) were shown a selection of fourteen clips from interviews of suspects in their

    police interviews (data collected from Mann, Vrij, & Bull, 2002). All participants

    observed the same clips but one group saw the video with no audio, another group heard

    the audio of the clips but saw no video image. The third (control) group saw the video as

    normal. In Experiments 1, 3, and 5, 255 undergraduate students participated: 120 in

    Experiment 1, 80 in Experiment 3 and 55 in Experiment 5. In Experiments 2, 4, 6, and 7,

    290 police officers participated: 68 in Experiment 2, 55 in Experiment 4, 64 in

    Experiment 6, and 103 in Experiment 7.

    Results

    Concentrating on Visual Cues

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    11/24

    RES-000-23-0292 32

    The truth accuracy, lie accuracy and response bias (B measured with the Signal

    detection Theory approach) of Experiment 7 are presented in Table 1. The higher the B

    score, the stronger the lie bias was.

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    12/24

    RES-000-23-0292 33

    Table 1. Truth Accuracy, Lie Accuracy and Response Bias (B) as a Function of Medium

    Exposure

    Condition Truth accuracy Lie accuracy BM SD M SD

    Visual + audio (N = 35) .59 .1 .70 .1 .21

    Visual only (N = 37) .34 .2 .53 .2 .36

    Audio only (N = 31) .63 .2 .69 .1 .11

    Truth accuracy, lie accuracy and response bias all differed from each other (all

    F s > 3.38, all ps < .05, all eta2

    > .07. Posthoc tests regarding truth and lie accuracy

    revealed that, as predicted, the participants in the visual only condition achieved lower

    truth and lie accuracy rates than the participants in both of the other two conditions.

    Posthoc tests regarding the response bias revealed that participants in the visual condition

    made more lie judgements than participants in the audio condition. These findings

    suggest that observing visual cues results in a lie bias, as we predicted. The fact that a lie

    bias in the visual only condition did not result in high lie accuracy is interesting. It

    appears that more lie judgements (66%) were made when suspects were telling the truth

    than when they were lying (47%). This is probably the result of the inaccurate

    stereotypical view that observers have about how liars behave (Vrij, Akehurst, & Knight,

    2006). For example, although observers believe that liars make more movements than

    truth tellers, research has shown that truth tellers typically move more than liars. As a

    result, by concentrating on movements, the livelier participants (e.g., truth tellers) will be

    classified as liars.

    Baseline Method

    Analyses of Experiments 5 and 6 are still in progress. Preliminary results show

    mixed support for our hypothesis that having a meaningful baseline will facilitate lie

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    13/24

    RES-000-23-0292 34

    detection. In Experiment 5 we first examined the behavioural differences between liars

    and truth tellers whilst they guessed how many dots there were on the screen (target

    period). At a multivariate level, the analysis did not reveal a significant multivariate

    effect, F (13, 41) = 1.18, ns. Univariate tests only revealed one significance: Liars paused

    longer ( M = .89 per minute of speech, SD = 1.2) than truth tellers ( M = .30, SD = .6), F (1,

    53) = 4.60, p < .05, eta 2 = .08. This analysis resembles the task lie detectors face when

    they are required to distinguish between truths and lies when no meaningful baseline is

    available.

    We then carried out a second analysis. This time we detracted the behavioural

    scores from liars and truth tellers during the target period from their behavioural scores

    during the meaningful control period. This analysis revealed a significant multivariate

    effect, F (13, 41) = 2.10, p < .05, eta 2 = .40. Five significant effects emerged at a

    univariate level, comprising illustrators, hand/finger movements, self adaptors, gaze

    aversion and blinks, all F s > 2.86, all ps < .05, all eta 2 > 05. For each behaviour, liars

    displayed larger differences between the target period and the meaningful control period

    than truth tellers: Liars decreased their movements more and showed more gaze aversion

    than truth tellers. This analysis resembles the task lie detectors face when they are

    required to detect truths and lies when there is a meaningful baseline available. In

    summary, when a meaningful baseline was available more cues (five) discriminated

    between truth tellers and liars than when such a baseline was not available (one).

    In Experiment 6 police officers were shown a sample of the liars and truth tellers

    from Experiment 5 either with a meaningful baseline (experimental condition) or without

    a meaningful baseline available (control condition). Both groups obtained around a 60%

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    14/24

    RES-000-23-0292 35

    accuracy rate. A possible explanation why the experimental participants did not

    outperform the control participants is that they misinterpreted the diagnostic cues. The

    liars showed a decrease in movements, which is typically found in deception research,

    whereas observers widely believe that liars increase their movements. Perhaps

    experimental participants will outperform control participants if they are informed

    beforehand that liars typically decrease rather than increase their movements. We are

    going to test this hypothesis in a follow-up experiment.

    Behaviour Analysis Interview

    The article concerning the Behaviour Analysis Interview experiment (Experiment

    1) is attached, so please see that article for further details (Vrij, Mann, & Fisher, 2006a).

    The results revealed that liars were less co-operative and appeared more nervous than

    truth tellers. These results were consistent with our cognitive load/impression

    management hypothesis in direct opposition to Inbau et al.s predictions. When in a

    subsequent lie detection experiment (Experiment 2), police officers were shown those

    interviews based on the Behaviour Analysis Interview, they performed no better than

    chance in discriminating between liars and truth tellers (Vrij, Mann, Kristen, & Fisher, in

    press, this article is also attached).

    Cognitive Load Approach: Information-gathering Interview Accusatory Interview

    Comparison

    When we compared participants experiences during the information-gathering

    interviews and the accusatory interviews (Experiment 1) we found, as predicted, that

    information-gathering interviews were perceived as more cognitively demanding ( M =

    4.23, SD = 1.3) than accusatory interviews ( M = 3.36, SD = 1.5), F (1, 76) = 7.36, p < .01,

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    15/24

    RES-000-23-0292 36

    eta 2 = .09 (Vrij, Mann, & Fisher, 2006b). Moreover, information-gathering interviews

    resulted, as predicted, in more verbal cues to deceit than accusatory interviews (Vrij,

    Mann, Kristen, & Fisher, in press, attached article). The detection of deception

    experiment (Experiment 2) did not reveal differences in truth and lie accuracy when the

    information-gathering and accusatory interviews were compared (Vrij, Mann, Kristen, &

    Fisher, in press, attached article), however, accusatory interviews resulted in more false

    accusations (classifying a truth teller as a liar) than information-gathering interviews.

    Cognitive Load Approach: The Reverse Order Experiments

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    16/24

    R E S

    - 0 0 0

    - 2 3

    - 0 2 9 2

    3 7

    T a b

    l e 2 . V e r

    b a l , V o c a l a n

    d V i s u a

    l C u e s a s a

    F u n c t

    i o n o f

    V e r a c

    i t y f o r

    t h e

    R e v e r s e

    O r d e r a n

    d C o n

    t r o l

    C o n

    d i t i o n s

    S e p a r a

    t e l y

    R e v e r s e

    O r d e r

    C o n

    t r o l

    C o n

    d i t i o n

    T r u

    t h

    L i e

    F ( 1 , 3 8 )

    E t a

    2

    T r u

    t h

    L i e

    F ( 1 , 3 8 )

    e t a 2

    M

    S D

    M

    S D

    M

    S D

    M

    S D

    A u d

    i t o r y

    d e t a i l s

    9 . 0 0

    4 . 6

    5 . 3 5

    3 . 6

    7 . 6 2 * * *

    . 1 7

    6 . 2

    5

    4 . 2

    8 . 8 5

    4 . 2 3 . 8 0

    . 0 9

    C o n

    t e x t u a

    l e m

    b e d d i n g s

    1 8 . 9 5

    7 . 3

    1 2 . 1

    5

    5 . 9

    1 0 . 5

    1 * * *

    . 2 2

    1 7 . 0

    5

    1 0 . 3

    1 7 . 1

    0

    7 . 4

    . 0 0

    . 0 0

    U n s

    t r u c

    t u r e

    d p r o d u c

    t i o n

    6 . 6 5

    2 . 0

    5 . 5 0

    2 . 0

    3 . 3 6 *

    . 0 8

    . 1 0

    . 3

    . 1 5

    . 4 . 2

    2

    . 0 1

    S p e e c h

    h e s i

    t a t i o n s

    4 . 5 4

    2 . 6

    5 . 9 6

    2 . 0

    3 . 8 6 *

    . 0 9

    4 . 3

    6

    2 . 2

    3 . 9 9

    2 . 0

    . 3 2

    . 0 1

    S p e e c h r a

    t e

    1 6 3 . 6 2

    2 9 . 2

    1 4 2 . 1 3

    1 3 . 7

    5 . 1 5 * *

    . 1 2

    1 6 2 . 8

    4

    2 1 . 0

    1 6 9 . 0 2

    3 2 . 9

    . 5 0

    . 0 1

    H a n

    d / f i n g e r

    1 1 . 7 8

    9 . 8

    1 5 . 4

    1

    1 3 . 1

    . 9 8

    . 0 2

    1 4 . 5

    5

    8 . 7

    7 . 3 3

    6 . 4 8 . 8 4 * * *

    . 1 9

    L e g

    / f o o

    t

    5 . 2 1

    5 . 3

    1 3 . 8

    0

    1 6 . 9

    4 . 6 8 * *

    . 1 1

    1 4 . 9

    7

    2 1 . 1

    6 . 2 8

    5 . 8 3 . 1 5 ( . 0 8 )

    . 0 8

    C o g n i

    t i v e o p e r a t

    i o n s

    1 . 7 5

    2 . 0

    4 . 3 5

    4 . 6

    5 . 3 0 * *

    . 1 2

    2 . 0

    5

    3 . 9

    3 . 6 5

    3 . 4 1 . 9 3

    . 0 5

    S p e e c h e r r o r s

    1 . 2 6

    0 . 7

    1 . 9 1

    1 . 2

    3 . 8 7 *

    . 0 9

    1 . 4

    5

    1 . 2

    1 . 5 1

    1 . 2

    . 0 2

    . 0 0

    E y e

    b l i n k s

    1 7 . 3 8

    9 . 7

    2 7 . 0

    7

    1 2 . 7

    7 . 3 4 * * *

    . 1

    1 9 . 5

    6

    9 . 1

    2 4 . 7

    5

    1 2 . 1 2 . 3 4

    . 0 6

    * p

    < . 0

    5 , o n e

    t a i l e d t e s t

    * * p

    < . 0

    5 , t w o

    t a i l e d t e s t

    * * * p

    < . 0

    1 , t w o -

    t a i l e d t e s t

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    17/24

    Table 2 reveals the verbal and nonverbal cues that differentiated liars from

    truth tellers in the reverse order and control conditions (Experiment 3). The higher the

    score, the more frequently the cues occurred. The cues are measured per 100 words

    (speech hesitations and speech errors) or per minute of interview (the remaining

    cues). As predicted, many more cues to deceit emerged in the reverse order condition

    than in the control condition and most of the cues that distinguished liars from truth

    tellers in the reverse order condition are signs of cognitive load (all cues except

    increase in foot/leg movements and eye blinks).

    In Experiment 4, we showed police officers a random sample of these reverse

    order and control interviews and examined their ability to detect deceit in those

    interviews.

    Table 3. Truth Accuracy, Lie Accuracy and Response Bias (B) as a Function of Reverse

    Order

    Condition Truth accuracy Lie accuracy BM SD M SD

    Reverse Order (N = 31) .56 .2 .60 .2 .07

    Control (N = 24) .50 .2 .42 .2 -.13

    Lie accuracy in the Reverse Order condition was significantly higher than lie

    accuracy in the control condition, F (1, 53) = 10.53, p < .01, eta 2 = .17. Lie accuracy in

    the Reverse Order condition was significantly above the level of chance, t (30) = 2.68,

    p < .05, whereby the lie accuracy in the control condition was significantly below the

    level of chance, t (23) = 1.97, p < .05. Although the truth accuracy in the Reverse

    Order condition (56%) was also higher than the truth accuracy in the control condition

    (50%), this difference was not significant, F (1, 53) = 1.26, ns, eta 2 = .02. The

    difference in response bias was not significant either between the two conditions, F (1,

    38

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    18/24

    53) = .87, ns, eta 2 = .02. Neither of the two response biases differed significantly from

    zero, t s < 1.59, p > .12. In other words, instructing interviewees to tell their stories in

    reverse order led to higher lie accuracy without impairing truth accuracy, without

    influencing the response bias.

    In summary, the project showed that several methods traditionally used by

    investigators to distinguish between truths and lies do not work. However, our new

    cognitive load approach showed potential. Cognitive load interview protocols are

    designed to make information-gathering interviews more demanding for suspects.

    This increased demand has a larger impact on liars than on truth tellers because liars

    already find being interviewed more mentally taxing than truth tellers. The result is

    that cognitive load interview protocols facilitate discrimination between liars and truth

    tellers.

    Activities

    The results of Experiments 1, 2, 3, 4, and 7 have been presented at European

    and American Psychology and Law conferences. The lead applicant, Aldert Vrij,gives detection of deception talks worldwide to both practitioners and scholars

    (around ten presentations a year) and the results of this ESRC project have a

    prominent place in these presentations. Moreover, Vrij is currently updating his

    Telling and detecting lies book (originally published in 2000) and the results of this

    ESRC project play an important role in the revised version of his book.

    Outputs

    The project has so far resulted in four publications (Law and Human Behavior

    is the most prestigious Psychology and Law Journal):

    Vrij, A., & Mann, S. (2006). Criteria-Based Content Analysis: An empirical test

    of its underlying processes. Psychology, Crime, & Law , 12, 337-349.

    39

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    19/24

    Vrij, A., Mann, S., & Fisher, R. (2006a). An empirical test of the Behaviour

    Analysis Interview. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 329-345.

    Vrij, A., Mann, S., & Fisher, R. (2006b). Information-Gathering vs Accusatory

    Interview Style: Individual Differences in Respondents' Experiences. Personality and

    Individual Differences , 41, 589-599.

    Vrij, A., Fisher, R., Mann, S., & Leal, S. (2006). Detecting deception by

    manipulating cognitive load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 10, 141-142.

    One more publication is in press: Vrij, A., Mann, S., Kristen, S., & Fisher, R.

    (in press). Cues to deception and ability to detect lies as a function of police interview

    styles. Law and Human Behavior .

    There are many more publications to come. One paper (Vrij, Mann, Fisher,

    Leal, Milne, & Bull) regarding Experiments 3 and 4 has been submitted, and we will

    submit a paper regarding Experiment 7 soon. We further received invitations to write

    one book chapter about the project (to appear in International developments in

    investigative interviewing (Milne, Savage, & Williamson, Eds.), and two entries in theSage Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law (Cutler, Ed.). Experiments 5 and 6 still need

    to be written up, as well as the nonverbal data of Experiment 1 and the individual

    differences data from Experiments 1 and 2.

    Impacts

    Throughout this project we worked closely together with the policeconstabularies of Hampshire and Kent and they showed interest in the results. Once

    all the data of the project have been fully analysed and written up (we expect this

    work to be completed within the next six months) we will contact those constabularies

    to discuss the findings. We are planning to discuss the findings also in other

    constabularies, and the strong links the co-applicants Milne and Bull have with

    40

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    20/24

    various police constabularies in England and Wales are thereby particularly helpful.

    The project has also received interest from several Governments (UK and abroad)

    with whom we work on lie detection issues.

    Future Research Priorities

    We believe that our cognitive load detection of deception approach has

    potential and are currently conducting research to refine this approach. Vrij, Fisher,

    and Mann received a three-year grant (not from the ESRC) to sponsor these research

    activities. The nature of these activities is confidential and cannot be disclosed. Dr

    Samantha Mann, who worked as a Research Fellow on the ESRC project, has been

    employed as a Research Fellow on this new grant.

    Ethics

    No ethical considerations have arisen from the project. Students enjoyed

    participating because they like to see whether they are good liars. Police officers were

    sometimes less enthusiastic, because they did not always see the relevance of the

    experiments. The problem is that, as participants allocated to specific experimental

    conditions, they lack the overview regarding the aims of the experiments. The

    debriefing clarified these issues, but the hypotheses under investigation were

    sometimes received with skepticism. Not a single participant, however, withdrew

    during the experiments or asked his/her data to be withdrawn after the experiment.Because each experiment demanded so much time from police participants (on

    average just over an hour to run) and we generally recruited participants from police

    training colleges where courses are highly structured without much free time, it took a

    lot of time to accumulate the number of police participants that we did. Therefore we

    recruited no more participants then strictly essential to test our hypotheses.

    41

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    21/24

    Confidentiality

    Nothing reported in this End of Reward Report is sensitive or confidential.

    Number of words: 4996

    42

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    22/24

    References

    Bond, C. F., & DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Accuracy of deception judgements.

    Personality and Social Psychology Review , 10, 214-234.

    Buller, D. B., & Burgoon, J. K. (1996). Interpersonal deception theory.

    Communication Theory , 6 , 203-242.

    DePaulo, B. M., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Nonverbal communication. In D. T.

    Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 3-40).

    Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.

    DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. L., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K.

    & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin , 129 , 74-118.

    Ekman, P. (1985). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics and

    marriage . New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

    Fisher, R. P., Brennan, K. H., & McCauley, M. R. (2002). The cognitive

    interview method to enhance eyewitness recall. In M. L. Eisen, J. A. Quas, & G. S.

    Goodman (Eds.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview (pp. 265-286).Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist ,

    46 , 107-119.

    Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The psychology of interrogations and confessions .

    Chichester, England: Wiley & Sons.

    Inbau, F. E., Reid, J. E., Buckley, J. P., & Jayne, B. C. (2001). Criminal

    interrogation and confessions, 4th edition . Gaithersburg, Maryland: Aspen Publishers.

    Kassin, S. M. (2005). On the psychology of confessions: Does innocence put

    innocents at risk? American Psychologist , 60, 215-228.

    43

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    23/24

    Kassin, S. M., & Norwick, R. J. (2004). Why people waive their Miranda

    rights: The power of innocence. Law and Human Behavior , 28, 211-221.

    Mann, S., Vrij, A., & Bull, R. (2002). Suspects, lies and videotape: An

    analysis of authentic high-stakes liars. Law and Human Behavior , 26 , 365-376.

    Mann, S. & Vrij, A. (2006). Police officers' judgements of veracity, tenseness,

    cognitive load and attempted behavioural control in real life police interviews.

    Psychology, Crime, & Law , 12, 307-319.

    Masip. J., Sporer, S., Garrido, E., & Herrero, C. (2005). The detection of

    deception with the reality monitoring approach: A review of the empirical evidence.

    Psychology, Crime, & Law , 11 , 99-122.

    Moston, S., & Engelberg, T. (1993). Police questioning techniques in tape

    recorded interviews with criminal suspects. Policing and Society , 3, 223-237.

    National Research Council (2003). The polygraph and lie detection .

    Committee to Review the Scientific Evidence on the Polygraph (2003). Washington,

    DC: The National Academic Press.Spence, S. A., Hunter, M. D., Farrow, T. F. D., Green, R. D., Leung, D. H.,

    Hughes, C. J., & Ganesan, V. (2004). A cognitive neurobiological account of

    deception: Evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philosophical Transactions of the

    Royal Society of London, 359 , 1755-1762.

    Vrij, A. (1995). Behavioral correlates of deception in a simulated police

    interview. Journal of Psychology , 129 , 15-28.

    Vrij, A. (2000). Detecting lies and deceit: The psychology of lying and its

    implications for professional practice . Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.

    Vrij, A. (2004). Invited article: Why professionals fail to catch liars and how they

    can improve. Legal and Criminological Psychology , 9, 159-181.

    44

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC

  • 8/13/2019 RES-000-23-0292-5k (1)

    24/24

    Vrij, A. (2005). Criteria-Based Content Analysis: A qualitative review of the

    first 37 studies. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law , 11 , 3-41.

    Vrij, A. (2006). Challenging interviewees during interviews: The potential

    effects on lie detection. Psychology, Crime, & Law , 12, 193-206.

    Vrij, A., Akehurst, L., & Knight, S. (2006). Police officers', social workers',

    teachers' and the general public's beliefs about deception in children, adolescents and

    adults. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 11, 297-312.

    Vrij, A., Mann, S., & Fisher, R. (2006a). An empirical test of the Behaviour

    Analysis Interview. Law and Human Behavior, 30, 329-345.

    Vrij, A., Mann, S., & Fisher, R. (2006b). Information-Gathering vs Accusatory

    Interview Style: Individual Differences in Respondents' Experiences. Personality and

    Individual Differences , 41, 589-599.

    Vrij, A., Mann, S., Kristen, S., & Fisher, R. (in press). Cues to deception and

    ability to detect lies as a function of police interview styles. Law and Human Behavior .

    Vrij, A., Fisher, R., Mann, S., & Leal, S. (2006). Detecting deception bymanipulating cognitive load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 10, 141-142.

    Vrij, A., Semin, G. R., & Bull, R. (1996). Insight into behaviour during

    deception. Human Communication Research , 22, 544-562.

    Walczyk, J. J., Schwartz, J. P., Clifton, R., Adams, B., Wei, M., & Zha, P.

    (2005). Lying person-to-person about live events: A cognitive framework for lie

    detection. Personnel Psychology , 58, 141-170.

    45

    To cite this output:Vrij, Aldert (2007). Interviewing to Detect Deception: Full Research Report.ESRC End of Award Report, RES-000-23-0292. Swindon: ESRC