23
KANAGAWA University Repository \n Title THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS : The Comprehension Process of Phrasal Verbs Author(s) Suzuki, Hiroko Citation �, 12: 1-22 Date 1990-03-01 Type Departmental Bulletin Paper Rights publisher

Suzuki, Hiroko Date Type Departmental Bulletin Paper ...klibredb.lib.kanagawa-u.ac.jp/dspace/bitstream/10487/3674/1/kana... · THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS : The Comprehension

  • Upload
    lyliem

  • View
    216

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

KANAGAWA University Repository

\n

TitleTHE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS : The

Comprehension Process of Phrasal Verbs

Author(s) Suzuki, Hiroko

Citation 神奈川大学言語研究, 12: 1-22

Date 1990-03-01

Type Departmental Bulletin Paper

Rights publisher

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC The Comprehension Process of Phrasal Verbs

VERBS

Hiroko Suzuki

INTORDUCTION

The children learn an average of 3, 000 new words every year. In

the first grade, 7 year-old children know 7, 000 words and they usually

learn 14, 000 words at the age of 14 (Richards, 1976). How can they

learn so many words ? They must be acquiring the words apart from

instruction specifically devoted to vocabulary learning (Nagy, 1987).

The developmental stages of vocabulary acquisition of young children

demonstrated a two-step learning process. In the first step, they assign

a new word to a semantic category. Then, they discover the disting-

uising particulars of the new word from the other words belonging to

the same category. That is, they assimilate the incoming word to and distinguish it from the other words in the related category which they

have constructed. They form conceptually related patterns and build

up their vocabulary as a coherent integrated system of concepts. This

process enables them to learn the words so rapidly (Miller, 1986). Miller (1986) introduced "semantic field" as an important concept

to explain the process of vocabulary acquisition. A semantic field is a

set of words whose semantic decompositions all share at least one

nuclear concept. The dictionary in your head (the knowledge of your

vocabulary) is organized into many sub-dictionaries for different se-

mantic fields, where any single semantic field has minimal polysymy.

On the other hand, the semantic unit is the organization of words'

meanings which the learner has learned in the past. This organization

unites what he/she knows about the different aspects of meaning

2

conveyed by the word in different contexts. Thus, understanding a

word in a particular context is sustained by the unit. The unit is

strenthened by processing the word's meaning in a new context and

adding new information to it (Elshout-Mohr, 1987). The more stren-

thened the unit, the more abstractly the learner grasp the word' s

meaning, the more efficiently the learner can automate the process

and the more flexibly he/she can use the word in various contexts.

Therefore, we have the organizations united the information both of

a single word's meanings and of the relationships among the words

in our vocabulary.

Some emprical studies have demonstrated that younger children

perceive words as `concrete' ideas, which emphasizes their isolated or particular aspects, whereas older children stress the abstract of `class' features of the word meanings

, both in a recall-type test, as

productive knowledge (Feifel and Lorge, 1950, Russell, 1962) and in a multiple-choice, recognition type test, as receptive knowledge

(Kruglore, 1953).

As well as the complexity of comprehension processes of learners,

we have to consider the diversity of the word's meaning when we

discuss the process of acquiring words. Carter (1987) tried to clarify

the definition of core vocabulary. The first characteristics is that

phrases of core words with adverbs or adjectives can be substituted. For instance, "laugh in a loud and rude manner" can substitute for 'guffaw'. The second characteristic of core vocabulary is collocability.

That is, `bright' can collocate with either `sun', `light,' `idea' or 'green

,' while `radiant' can with `light' but not with 'green'. The

collocability of `bright' is higher than 'radiant'. The core words also

extend their meanings to such forms as compounds, idioms and phrasal

verbs. Thus, the core words produce more meanings by forming

pharses. Lado (1964), on the other hand, pointed out the complexity of understanding the basic words due to the fact that they are used

in various contexts, so that they are sometimes used as particular

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS 3

meanings we are extremely unfamiliar with. The basic words, in a sense, don't necessarily have a single basic concept, but, rather, they

are quite flexible to express various meanings either independently or

followed by adverbs or prepositions in different contexts. Therefore

it must be difficult for the learners to grasp the entire meaning of

the basic words.

The frequency list of the words in the textbooks displayed that the

video-aided textbooks (spoken English) tend to use basic words more

frequently than the highschool textbooks for reading (written English)

(Kotani and Suzuki, 1988). That is, the vocabulary size of the spoken English material is smaller than the one of the written English

materials. Holliday (1988) explained that the spoken English is lexically

sparse but grammatically intricate compared with the written English.

The analysis of the target "basic verbs" tells that in the spoken

English textbooks the basic verbs used in different contexts had various

meanings, especially when they were accompanied with adverbs and

prepositions (Kotani and Suzuki, 1988). Thus in spoken English it is very important to grasp the context accurately and to pick one

appopriate meaning of the basic word from the context. It may be

one of the difficulties for the learners in understanding spoken English.

The purpose of this study was to investigate how much the Japanese

college students acquired conceptual meanings of the basic verbs which

were used as phrasal verbs. In this study, the students were given

the vocabulary tests to examine their knowledge of the phrasal verbs,

where the phrasal verbs in the tests retained the primary meanings

of the verbs themselves. These empirical vocabulary tests were com-

posed in order to analyze how the students use the basic verbs with

prepositions or adverbs to form the phrases, so that their acquisition level of the verbs is discussed, considering the comprehension process.

4

PROCEDURE

1) subjects

The subjects were 507 college students, mostly freshmen and so-

phomores. Three fourth of the subjects were studying Engineering and the rest were majoring in either Spanish or Law.

2) vocabulary test

Two sets of vocabulary tests totally consisting of 40 sentences were

made. The first task was to pick up the sentences from video-aided

textbooks and the books of communicative grammar and vocabulary.

The sentences chosen from the textbooks included the verb phrases

using seven out of the 13 basic verbs, which had been selected in the

previous study. The seven verbs were get, go, look, put, run, take and turn. The style of the tests was multiple choice. The subjects

were asked to fill in the blanks of the sentences by choosing the

appropriate word from 7 or 8 alternatives. The more alternatives there

are, the lower the probability of picking out the correct answer by

chance. They were also allowed to use these alternatives as many times

as they liked. In this way, it is assumed that the test examines their

productive knowledge of the target words. Out of 20 questions in each test, half of them required verbs and the other half, adverbs or

prepositions. The students were asked to mark their answers on marx cards, in order to simplify the scoring and to make efficient statistical

calculations of the data by computer. The words capitalized in [

were the synonyms or paraphrases of the target pharses. These clues

were added to the questions whose contexts were not clarified enough

to find the appropriate words for the blanks (See Appendix).

Immediately after collecting the vocabulary tests, the same tests

with the correct answers were distributed to the subjects (See Appen-

dix). They were assinged to write Japanese translations for the target

phrases in order to find out whether they could understand when they saw the target pharasal verbs in contexts, to determine their receptive

knowledge.

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS 5

RESULTS and DISCUSSION

The two sets of vocabulary tests were conducted twice respectively,

in January and April, 1989. The data between the January and April

tests were compared in order to examine the consistency of the

difficulty of each question.

Table 1 shows the mean in each test, where the possiblity of the

tests were all 20 points. The average score of the January test in

TEST B was higher than the other tests.

TABLE I. THE MEANS OF THE VOCABULARY TESTS

TEST A

TEST B

No. of Subjects V oc. Tests Japanese trans.

January

April

112-

98- 1-2107. 2 pts

7. 5 pts 14. 8 pts

January

April

141-

157- 1-2989. 8 pts

7. 7 pts 11. 8 pts

In order to take a look at the consistency of difficulty of each question,

between the January and April tests, the correlation coefficient (the

Pearson product moment correlation) was computed. The coefficiency

r.. 74 indicates the significance of the correlation between the data

of the January and the April tests (See Figures 1-1,4).

Interestingly enough, the inappropriate verbs chosen by more than

20% of the students were almost the same in both the January and

the April tests as well. For example, in Question 9, TEST A, the

sentence was "When did your neighbors (get) back from their holidays?"

In the January test, the correct answer rate was 27. 6%, the second

highest percentage of the verb chosen was go, at 34. 7%, and third

choice was turn, at 29. 0%, On the other hand, the correct answer

rate in the April test was 20. 5%, the second highest was also go, at

33. 0 % and the third one was turn, at 30. 4%.

From the correlational analysis, therefore, the data of the April

tests is considered consistant with the January tests and reliable for

further analyses (See Table 2).

6

un 44 g W

QQ

E-4 r W E-+

vz 1

CD i4 Z

• bd) T.4

(~zcn~wx c~~H 1 o )

•GET back

PUT up

RUN out

GET off

TAKE out

GO on

LOOK up

TURN down rZi

TAKE up

1

t

;i4

H E

O C) CO t•-• cD

(¢zcnwx

O O O 0 LCD CO CV

¢Fw

o0

i

w

Cin

EH

E

1

bo

Q~coc10d'Mc<=3

(e4Zci WG4 E-iW \)

GET throu

.GET up

LOOK up

GO back

PUT away

GET on

TURN up

RUN down

TURN on

TAKE off

a

P4

E-+

W

w (Qzt/Z,~~Wx \)

•run ON

go THROU

get OVER

put BACK

take DOWN

go OVER

turn OUT

take OFF

look ROUND

put OUT

look DOWN

x

THE

TABLE 2 THE ANSWER

ACQUISITION OF

RATES OF THE

THE BASIC

VOCABULARY

VERBS

TESTS

[TEST A] TOTAL JAN. APR.

1. TAKE off

(PUT) 2. TURN on

(PUT)

(TAKE)

(GET) 3. RUN down

4. TURN up

(LOOK) 5. GET on

6. PUT away

(TAKE) 7. Go back

8. LOOK up

(TURN) 9. GOT back

(GO)

(TURN)

10. GET through

(RUN) 11. look BACK

12. get OVER

(ON)

(DOWN) 13. put DOWN

(ON)

14. take OVER

(AWAY)

(ROUND) 15. Iook ROUND

(OVER) 16. go BY

(AWAY)

(OFF) 17. get OFF

(AWAY) 18. turn OVER

(DOWN) 19. run AWAY

20. get ON

32. 0

50. 0

9. 5

26. 7

22. 4

21. 0

37. 6

38. 6

38. 1

55. 2

31. 4

32. 4

59. 5

13.8

36. 7

29. 5

33. 8

30. 0

39. 1

32. 4

59. 1

26. 7

33. 3

21. 0

40. 0

8. 1

29. 5

29. 5

46. 7

35.7

15.2

41. 0

21. 4

40. 0

24. 1

36. 7

22. 0

79.2

50. 0

29. 6

53. 1

8. 9

21. 4

29.4

/ 31. 6

40. 8

37. 8

61. 2

15. 3

45. 9

62. 2

12. 2

33. 7

27. 5

34. 7

29.6

34. 7

39. 8

53. 1

12.3

42. 7

21. 4

21. 4

40. 8

8. 2

27. 6

36. 7

56. 1

32. 7

15. 3

36. 7

21. 4

43. 9

22. 5

36. 7

22. 5

74. 5

49. 0

[TEST B] TOTAL JAN.

47.3

33. 9

10. 2

32. 7

24. 5

42. 9

36. 6

38. 4

50. 0

45. 5

20. 5

57. 1

15. 2

39.3

20. 5

33. 0

30. 4

42.9

25.9

64. 3

38.4

25. 0

20. 5

39. 2

8.0

31. 3

23. 2

38. 4

38. 4

15.8

44. 6

21. 4

36.6

31. 3

36. 6

28. 6

77. 7

50. 9

1. TAKE up

(PUT) 2. TURN down

(PUT) 3. LOOK up

(TURN)

(RUN) 4. GO on

5. TAKE out

6. GET off

7. RUN out

S. PUT up

(GET)

GET back

(GO)

(TURN) 10. look DOWN

11. put OUT

(ON)

(DOWN)

12. look ROUND

(OVER)

13. take OFF

14. turn OUT

15. go OVER

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

(THROUGH) take DOWN

(ON)

put BACK

get OVER

(THROUGH)

go THROUGH run ON

(OVER)

48. 7

25. 8

65. 4

/

20. 1

20. 1

20. 5

66.4

66. 1

69. 5

43. 3

42. 0

23. 5

18. 5

40. 3

28. 9

65. 4

14. 1

36. 2

26. 2

41. 9

27.5

66.4

45. 3

22. 1

58. 1

37.2

23. 5

70. 1

25. 8

47. 3

40. 3

32.2

22. 8

46. 4

28. 6

88.6

/ 25. 0

23. 6

69. 3

79. 3

82.8

52. 9

52. 9

/ 20. 7

37. 1

29. 3

61. 1

44. 3

32. 7

47. 1

25. 7

79.3

47. 1

24. 3

64.3

45. 0

82. 1

31.4

52.9

45. 7

50. 7

7

C~o)

APR.

51.0

22. 9

45. 2

22. 9

15. 9

22.3

64. 3

54. 8

57. 9

35.3

32. 5

28. 0

16. 6

43.3

28. 0

63. 7

19. 8

42. 7

37. 6

29. 3

55. 4

43.9

15. 3

52.9

30. 6

29. 2

59. 9

20. 4

42. 7

35. 7

15. 9

27. 4

8

a) Receptive vs. Productive knowledge As Lado (1964) explains, our recognition vocabulary is much larger

than our productive vocabulary in the cognitive views of comprehen-

sion. In this experiment, the scores of the vocabulary tests for their

productive knowledge and the comprehension check by the Japanese translation for their receptive knowledge of the target pharasl verbs

were compared (See Figures 2-1,--,4).

T--tests (See Table 3) significantly show the superiority of the com-

prehension check test, suggesting that receptive knowledge is greater than the productive knowledge. This means, the subjects recognize

the meanings when they see the target verb phrases but they can't recall them in the appropriate context.

TABLE 3 DIFFERENCE OF ANSWER RATES BETWEEN THE VOCABULARY TESTS AND

THE JAPANESE COMPREHENSION CHECK

TEST A VOC

COMP

TEST B VOC

COMP

n x S tob.

112 37.9

76. 7

18. 25

18. 79

5. 68*

157 38. 6

59. 1

17.42

18. 30

4. 47*

*p> 0. 005

b) The comparison between the good vs. poor students determined by the vocabulary test scores

In order to find out the difficulty of the concepts of the target basic

verbs, the error analysis was taken by comparing the two groups

which were divided by the vocabulary test scores. The subjects who

got more than 9 points (Group A) or less than 6 points (Group B) were chosen as comparison group. The answer rates of each question

in the two groups were compared, to find out what factors had made

the questions difficult for Group B.

Figures 3-1r 4 compare the correct answer rates between Group A

and Group B for each question. According to the figures, questions

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS 9

V1

rit

a>

r r.t

w

C.)

1

fy CJ

it

O

8mo°o°.c°nc°-»c°~c. s f~zcn*wx x.tHw o }

GET back

PUT up 4'

RUN out

GET off T, L7-1

TAKE out

GO onCr) vi

LOOK up

TURN down ~G

t=: TAKE up

A

eti E-1

W E-+

cu a)

0

~

w

$,

Ci

cc

C')

(~: CID

°°01 C7, o

run ON

go THROU

get OVER

put BACK

take DOWN

go OVER

turn OUT

take OFF

look ROUND

put OUT

look DOWN

Ut :~hJL

w 1

w

Gy a) I.'

~0

w

a)

vs

U, C

U Gi

0 U

d 8 W ti 8 M N© C~

(r4zcnw1 x.e4h4.1 n )

GET through s

GET up ti

LOOK up

GO back

PUT away W GET on

TURN up cn

RUN down

TURN on E TAKE offe'

cd 0.4

•w

E-+

E-

Cl cu

6-

w

ctl

CJ F-^

0

oaaGot-cc °a+,c° *4

~x~hw

O

get ON

run AWAY

turn OVER

get OFF

go BY

look ROUND

take OVER

put DOWN

get OVER

.look BACK

cn

a

.幽国属

.〉

自く

"<

臼oo国臼

帆1θ⇔

㊥』ロ藷

聡 黙羅錘i璽§濫一"

r

'

「9・t・i'E'「¶圏寧 嗣■騨■■瞬一

綴 鱒ξ〃多饗.1■ 闇 「EI甲 ■

,fg¥

88 000000 ・、=)o』x)t、oしnr・0 1つ 〔・牙r一 イ

H

(o寓o=)山 く)

ONα

α州~)

り〇一α

卜.[α

燈円~)

め州

H,州α

η[α

斜囲α

一δ

O

自「一… 一一一

}灘i遊 疎ミ1画1■lllll

鍵O⇔

8

'=)o'⊂)(= .,(=)oし、OLO寸 ¢O⊂¥]

(o寓o=⊃ 山=q)

α

①δ

。。5

(のz9

隔め

「琵

))

卜δ

りδ

ぬ5

剤)

飾5

臼α

一δ§

㏄H

ritrathro≪.』『多i

N 9。thr⑪llZ'\

の笛属国

"<

臼の国臼

目-⇔◎

oh5嫡州角

/〆

ノ ノ ノ

90bacl{'ノ

1

纏 盤i§ §P・亡at・ayNi

戸「/

z

〃r,i多 蝕

-

蟹 \ 9・¥¥tO11...

1…藩tu。 面 。硫i彰:y/,%

・ ~ 〆

rundOwn 澄'\-畿1・

turnon// §§

takecuff〆 ノ 心 、 懸8謎08畠Qc,8禽 。

(o顛o⇔ 餌 く)

8

。。α

』」

8

8

δ

6うα

δ

δ

8同

runthrOU. εothY・ou'

一P'惜L」 ㎞一 己 晶

…;i蓬壷

//.'/'9。tも 。,1{

/ノ/∠ _4嘱亀 」∠

Iookbaε1ぞ グ

満冥「・彫 繍 ・k

麺 ・way、¥¥

ハt。rnd6、,7//of

ノ'/,

2)

・。α

δ

/

(。Qzo置

の国Dα)

8

δ

δ「

'

㎝、

、幽

驚顎

rundown¥ミ\ ///1.'

turnon

tikeoffw-i

・うα

⇔qα

δ

OQO

80自

OoaoOOO』oゆ 》oつ{NF→

(o名o⇔o,口q)

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS 1I

Qi W

g a

}~r

A rici

H W N

mti 1

co

cu N

0 GO

P1

cc

N. Cr

to

C1

-Cpl

///get back\put up \\\"~

run out

rik

16.get off\

Z77777,take`out ,4;,'.277

go on

look up

r

L

/take up/'4. ;iis I_-- i-- -- I I I I 1

C) o C) C) C') CC

O CD Ct C) C) C) C7 O t~ CD LC) ^C#' Cr) N r-i

(o P., <1)

Cr4 -Cr

00-C?

_a

C•1

-

/ rre,r,/,goz get back •I'

40

•":;/- put up4010.[fiP

pi run out '44Cv•vl^.>,)C

JC) 1.7":

e'outrovvi-ovi

707r/z.:A

7

[rrturn downNiv •4/ di&K1S^

take up IN

V

------------ N V

O a O O C, d Q O C, c) O C::, ~ co .C~- c,C) !f? d+ M CV r-i

(C C:).1 t4)

U) z 0

E-4 U2 W

0'

M

g W

etil

N r4 W E

co r

co a)

0

w

Cr4r-1

00

N-

- 514

1-4

CY)

ce

cqr-I

r-401.

..

go through

et over

ii\put• • •

back

take down.••

•••••• Nr- wiriir, •

take off :4+:4

Ilook round• • .,4-77- • • ..

put out

BP7-7r7:17o77-7-c 51 q'wn::<:,72A

. . . .

c= o o c> o c, c C) as GO t•• ccl If) Ntt+ cY? cNI

tc~xc )

C CW, CZ Ca C> C> d O O c co E- cflCY) cu

Cs.

00

C3r

cs,

.Z14 ci2 ~ W

M (~'

N r--I

I2

No. 3, 5, 6, 11, 12, 15, 18 and 20 in TEST A and questions No. 2, 5, 6,

7, 8, 13, 14, 16 and 19 in TEST B more obviously separate the two

groups than the other questions. These differences, first of all, indicate that Group A seemed to have learned the target phrases more accu-

rately than Group B. The phrases were presumably introduced as

idiomatic expressions in highschools as follows;

[e.g.]] put their toys (away)kft 6 get (over) a bad cold

look (round) the stationka1-

(turn) down the stereo <

(take) out three books 6

(get) off at the next stop 6 turn (out) to be a good experience r L b> 6

take (down) everything-'=1R6

The second significant difference is that Group A can apply the

concept of each verb and adverb/preposition to make a phrase.

[e.g. 2] tears (ran) down her face get (on) the horse

(put) up your handslook (back) on his childhood the boat turned (over)go (through) its narrow entrance

The verbs and the adverbs/prepositions of these phrases remain

their indivisual meanings. But they are not the verbs collocated with

adverbs and prepositions make particular meanings. The ability to

combine the verbs and adverbs/prepositions to use them appropriately

to the contexts differs depending on how much they understand the

concept of each word, that is, their acquisition levels of each word.

The vocabulary acquisition research on native language has concluded

that the older children tended to choose the abstract meaning of a

word while the younger children prefer the meaning fixed in a

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS 13

favorable or familiar context. It demonstrated, in the aspcet of developmental stages of vocabulary acquisition, that the depth of the

comprehension differed in the older and younger children (Feifel and

Large, 1950, Russel, 1962, Kruglore, 1953).

Thus, it follows that the students in Group A, who got higher

scores than the students in Group B, have bigger vocabularies. As

well as the vocabulary size, the cognitive levels of comprehension of

the target verbs and adverbs/prepositions differ in the two groups.

On the other hand, it is suspected, from the error analysis that

Group B tend to take two steps to come up with their answers. First

they tried to find out the concept that they had to convey in their

native language, and then, with the fixed expression in their native

language, they tried to find a word which has the common concept

from their knowledge of that word.

[e.g. 3] Mother told her children to (put) their toys away because it was bedtime.

26% of the subjects in Group B chose take instead of put here.

They tried to express "to take the things away to another place

in order to clear the table or floor."

[e. g. 4] I looked (round) the station but he couldn't see my friend anywhere.

From the Japanese translation of the target pharse, "j",

it is suspected that they tried to find the word expressing "'"",

resulting 37% of the subjects in Group B chose over.

[e. g. 5] The library allows you to (take) out three books at a time. The subjects presumably interpreted the Japanese translation of

the target phrase, "L ft 't", as the action that a person takes

the booK outside the library and look at them at home. 25% of

the subjects used look in this sentence.

[e.g. 6] The boat turned (over), throwing the boys into the water. 30% of the subjects chose down and 26% of them chose back

I4

in this sentence. Judging from their choice of verbs, it seems that

they tried to express the conditions or appearence of the boat

after it turned over, which the Japanese counterpart " <

also refers to. That is, they tried to deliver that the boat was

upside-down or that the bottom of the boat, which is like the

back of our body, appeared in the surface by turning over.

As these examples are demonstrated, we can easily suspect that the

subjects in Group B tend to translate the target phrase first and then

pick up the reasonable word from their translated meanings or concepts. The second point which displays their weakness was that they

seemed mixed up with the concepts or the use of the verbs among,

put, get and take. Here are three examples.

[e. g.7] Would you (turn) down the stereo ? I can't study. PUT

(23%) TAKE (25%)

[e. g. 8] If you would like to (get) off the next stop, push this button and the bus driver will notice it. TAKE (39%)

[e. g. 9] (put) up your hand, if you know the answer. GET (55 % )

However, this tendency can be seen not only in Group B but in all

the subjects. For example, in the sentence, "Mr. Black (takes) off his

glasses, because it is time to go to bed", 50% of the subjects chose

put instead of take. Thirdly, the subjects in Group B seemed to have difficulty to use

the phrases in a different context from the one they expect from the

fixed translated meaning of the target phrase.

[e. g.10] When I showed Kenichi some pictures, he seems to look

(back) on his childhood.

The correct answer rate of this sentence in Group B was 22%. If

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS 15

you understand the concept of "look back" and transmit the concept appropriately to the given context , you can predict that "look back"

also can be used as the meaning of "remember" . Actually, the answer rate of Group A on this question was pretty high (86%) .

This kind of conceptual transfer to a different conte xt must be difficult for the learners of foreign language , because the comprehension

by translating the words doesn't necessarily let the lear ners acquire syntactic and conceptual aspects of knowledge of the words as well. Thus as the following example shows , if the translation of the target phrase has implicitly a person as the subject , the learners hardly come up with the English counterpart in the sentence with an inanimate word as the subject.

[e. g.11] Business has been very bad lately but things are beginning to (look) up now.

The Japanese translation of "look up" is "J _EZ ", which is supposed to have a person as the subject in the Japanese structure , while in this sentence the subject is "business" . The correct answer rate of all the subjects at 18% shows the difficulty in understanding English as a subject-prominent language (Mitsutani , 1985) for the Japanese learners of English.

c) Interference by the transfer of Japanese translation

When the questions with low correct answer rates for all the subjects

were picked out, it was found out that the concepts of turn and over were confusing or ambiguous in these vocabulary tests .

[e. g. 12] The firemen (turned) the water on the next building to

put out the fire. PUT (28%) TAKE (22%)

[e. g. 13] The missing bag (turned) up, completely empty , in the lake. LOOK (38%)

I6

[e. g.14] Business has been very bad lately but things are beginning to (look) up now. TURN (37%)

[e. g.15] When did your neighbors (get) back from their holidays ? TURN (29%)

"Turn" in the example 12 means "to aim or point, set or direct

on a particular direction (Dictionary of Contemporary English,

Longman, 1987) ". Therefore, this sentence has to deliver that the

fireman changed the direction of the hoses. But the subjects

presumably could not find the word including this concept, so that they compromised to get a solution. Thus, over a half of the subjects

picked out either put (28%) to express that the firemen poured the water on the next building, or take (30%) to mean that the firemen

moved their hoses toward the next building.

The meaning of "turn" in the example 13 is "to (cause to) change

position or direction so as to face or move in a particular direction

(DCE, Lonman, 1987) ". The dictionary presents two kinds of sentences to introduce this

concept.

[1] She turned away and began to cry.

[1]' When they reached the boarder, they were turned back because

they had no passports.

[2] Our luck has turned.

It is noted when we translate [1] and [2], their meanings are ex-

pressed very differently in Japanese, while in English, "turn" in the

sentence [2] is merely used as a more abstract concept than the

sentence [1], according to the dictionary.

Thus it is predicted that the example 13 is difficult for the subjects.

In the exmaple 14, on the other hand, the subjects oversimplified

this concept. But actually "look up" is an informal idiomoatic phrase.

In the example 15, the subjects wanted to convey the meaning that "they changed the direction" . However, "get back home" means "not

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS 17

merely change the direction against where they had been heading

for," but "return to their original place ," so that the meanings were slightly different.

[e.g. 16] Why don't you take the children (over) to their grand-parents' house ? AWAY (30%) ROUND (30%)

[e. g.17] It's terribly noisy here, with planes going (over) our heads all the time. THROUGH (58%)

[e. g.18] Many cars are running in this street. How can we get

(over) ? THROUGH (47%)

Over also seemed to include complicated concepts . "Over" in the example 16 means "across an edge , distance, or an open air". The subjects must grasp the concept that they (are willing to) go to their

grandparents' house although it takes time. Therefore in order to express the distance, 30% of the subjects chose "away ."

Over of the example 17 and 18 were mixed up with through . Over in the exmple 17 means "directly above: higher than , but not touching." over in the example 18 refers to "from side to side of, expecially by going up and then down again." Through has a meaning of "in at one side end or surface of (something) and out at the other ." The direct translation of "go over" in the example 17 and "get over" in

the example 18 can be both "A6". "3&6" includes part of the

concepts of "over" and "through," that is, r k Z ~> -Na ` and 14t L < (Koizumi et al., 1989). However

the Japanese counterpart to over doesn't include the meaning "not

touching".

Therefore, it is obvious that the learners have difficulty in distin-

guishing the concept of "over" from "through" only by the translated meanings.

Lastly, there are two examples demonstrated that their vocabulary

acquisition process is interfered with the rote-memory of the fixed

IS

translation of the target phrases.

[e. g.19] Put (dowm) your suitcases. You can leave them for a while. ON (40%)

[e. g. 20:] I put the garden chairs (out) when the sun shone but soon had to take them in. ON (36%)

In the example 19 and 20, the subjects presumably came up with

the meaning of "-i r < " in Japanese. And the error analysis clearly

shows that the concepts of "E<" has just one phrasal expression "put on" in their minds .

CONCLUSIONS

The article reviews of the vocabulary acquisition process indicates

that the importance of learning words associated with contexts. As

far as the use of basic verbs is concerned, the flexibility to use the

verbs in various contexts must be one of the keys to expand the

expressions in spoken English.

The target phrasal verbs in the tests were not idiomatic expressions

for native speakers of English. Thus, one native English teacher

wondered what made "put the chair out in the garden" more difficult

than "put the glass on the table" for the subjects, because both sent-

ences simply have "put" followed by either a preposition or an adverb.

On the other hand, for Japanese learners of English "put on" is

regarded as the collocation or the idiom which can be associated with

Japanese translation "j < ". The present empirical study demonstrated, in this sense, that the

learners tend to be interfered by the fixed concepts of the target

verbs which they have learned with Japanese translation, when they

try to activate their knowledge about the target verbs to use them in

various context. What was lack in their vocabulary building process was that they

haven't learned the

contexts accompanied

words merely told the

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS 19

syntactic knowledge and linguistic and social

with the target verbs. The translation of the

learners the semantic information.

References Allsop, Jake. English Grammar. Cassell, 1983. Baxter, James. "The Dictionary and Vocabulary Behavior: a Single Word

or a Handful ?," TESOL Quarterly, 1980, 14, 3, 325-336. Carter, Ronald. "The notion of core vocabulary", in Vocabulary, Alen &

Unwin Ltd.: London, 1987, 33-46. Clark, Eve V. "Building a vocabulary: Words for objects, actions and rela-

tions," in Language Acquisition, (eds.) Paul Fletcher and Michael Garman, Cambridge University Press, 1979, 140-160.

Burtis, Mary E. "Vocabulary testing and vocabulary instruction," in The Nature of Vocabulary Acquisition, 1987, 37-52.

Elshout-Mohr, Marianne. "Cognitive processes in learning word meanings," in The Nature of Vocabulary Acquisition, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Hillsdale 1987, 53-71.

Feifel, Herman and Irving Lorge. "Qualitative differences in the vocabulary responses of children," The Journal of Educational Psychology, 1950,

41, 1, 1-18. Gerrig, Richard J. "Process and products of lexical access, Language and

Cognitive Processes, 1986, 1, 3, 187-195. Griffin, Suzanne. Follow me to San Francisco, BBC, 1981. Halliday, M.A.K. Spoken and Written Language. Oxford University Press,

1989. Judd, Elliott L. "Vocabulary Teaching and TESOL : A Need For Reevaluation

of Existing Assumptions," TESOL Quarterly, 1978, 12, 1, 71-129. 'J`WJJIs Vq% oD ;K-l5c 4l 1` 1985. 'J`~ lf~ Qlp~7aR1~'f~, 1989.

Kotani, Yukiko and Hiroko Suzuki. "Vocabulary Acquisition of Japanese College Students: Analyses on 13 Basic Verbs," Research Reports of

The Faculty of Engineering, Tokyo Denki University, 1988, 7. Kruglov, L.P. "Qualitative differences in the vocabulary choices or children

as revealed in a multiple choice test." Journal of Educational Psychology,

1953, 44, 229-243. Lado, Robert. Linguistics across cultures. The University of Michigan, 1964. Li, Xialong. "Effect of contextual cues on inferring and remembering meanings

of new words," Applied Linguistics, 1988, 9, 4, 403-413. Meara, Paul and Barbara Buxton. "An alternative to multiple choice

vocabulary test," Language Testing, 1987, 4, 2, 142-154. Miller, Geroge A. "Dictionaries in the mind," Language and Cognitive

20

Processes, 1986, 1, 3, 1986.

Acri rFiaL' =L LVDAMJ -, 1985. Munsell, Paul and Thomas H. Carr. "Monitoring the Monitor: A Review of

Second Language Acquisition and Second Language Learning. Stephen D. Krashen. Fair view Park: Pergamon Press, 1981. 252pp.," Language

Learning, 1981, 31, 2, 493-502. Murphy, R. English Grammar in Use. Cambridge University Press, 1987. Nagy, William E. and Patricia A. Herman. "Breadth and depth of vocabulary

knowledge: Implications for acquisition and instruction," in the Nature of Vocabulary Acquisition, 1987, 19-36.

Nilsen, Don L.F. "Contrastive Semantics in Vocabulary Instruction," TESOL Quarterly, 1976, 10, 1, 99-103.

Palmberg, Rolf. "Vocabulary Teaching in the Foreign-Language Classroom," Forum, 1986, 24, 3, 15-20.

Pertersen, Mark. Q H 4AGOi1A ;---; 0- ff , 1988. Read, John. "Measuring the Vocabulary Knowledge of Second Language

Learners," RELC Journal, 1988, 19, 2, 12-25. Richards, Jack C. "The Role of Vocabulary Teaching," TESOL Quarterly,

1976, 10, 77-79. Russell, D.H. and I.Q. Saadeh. "Qualitative levels in children's vocabularies,"

Journal of Educational Psychology, 1962, 53, 170-174. Schileifer, Aliah. "Reaching Out: A Strategy for Advanced Vocabulary

Acquisition," Forum, 1985, 23, 2, 11-15. Shimaoka, Takashi. "Robert Lado, Teaching English Across Cultures, 1988,

McGraw-Hill, Inc., IX 305 pp.," JAAL Bulletin, Japan Association of Applied Linguistics in JACET, 1989, 3. May.

[APPENDIX] [Vocabulary Building : TEST B]

Choose the right words.

a) Please (1) these letters up to the main office on the fifth floor. [CARRY * TO A HIGHER LEVEL]

b) Would you (2) down the stereo ? I can' t study. c) Business has been very bad lately but things are beginning to (3) up

now. [IMPROVE]

d) The fall of snow has blocked the road, so we can't (4) on any further. [CONTINUE]

e) The library allow you to (5) out three books at a time.

f) If you would like to (6) off at the next stop, push this button and the bus driver will notice it. [LEAVE A VEHICLE]

g) I went to the shop for some milk but they had (7) out. [NO LONGER

THE ACQUISITION OF THE BASIC VERBS

HAVE A SUPPLY]

h) (8) up, your hand, if you know the answer. [RAISE] 1) When did your neighbors (9) back from their holidays?

21

1. get 2. go 3. look 4. put 5. take 6. turn 7. run

a) Looking [10] at my foot, I saw my ring which I had lost for a few

days.

b) I put the garden chairs [11] when the sun shone, but soon had to take them in again because it began raining.

c) I look [12] the station but couldn't see my friend anywhere. [SEARCH]

d) A lot of TV stars and singers take [13] for Hawaii as soon as they have finished work in the end of the year. [LEAVE]

e) The accident has turned [14] to be a good experience for the boys after all. [RESULT]

f) It's terribly noisy here, with planes going [15] our heads all the time.

[PASS] g) Mr Cooper's secretary took [16] everything that the chairman said at

the meeting. [RECORD * IN WRITING]

h) When you have finished with the book, put it [17] on the shelf. [RETURN]

i) Many cars are running in this street. How can we get [18] ? [CROSS A ROAD]

j ) The piano won't go [19] its narrow entrance, it will have to come [*] the window.

k) This hair-drier runs [20] very little elecricity.

1. back 2. down 3. off 4. out 5. over 6. on 7. round 8. through

[Vocabulary Building: TEST B]

VDUt Lo

[A] 1) Please take these letters up to the main office on the fifth floor. 2) Would you turn down the stereo? I can't study. 3) Business has been very bad lately but things are beginning to look up

now.

4) The fall of snow has blocked the road, so we can' t go on any further. 5) The library allow you to take out three books at a time. 6) If you would like to get off at the next stop, push this button and the

22

bus driver will notice it.

7) 1 went to the shop for some milk but they had run out. 8) Put up your hand, if you know the answer.

9) When did your neighbors get back from their holidays ? [B] 10) Looking down at my foot, 1 saw my ring which I had lost for a few

days. 11) I put the garden chairs out when the sun shone, but soon had to take

them in again because it began raining.

12) I look round the station but couldn't see my fried anywhere. 13) A lot of TV stars and singers take off for Hawaii as soon as they

have finished work in the end of the year. 14) The accident has turned out to be a good experience for the boys after all. 15) It's terribly noisy here, with planes going over our heads all the time. 16) Mr Cooper's secretary took down everything that the chairman said at

the meeting.

17) When you have finished with the book, put it back on the shelf. 18) Many cars are running in this street. How can we get over ? 19) The piano won't go through its narrow entrance, it will have to come

through the window. 20) This hair-drier runs on very little elecricity.

1..

5.

9.

13.

17.

2.

6.

10.

14.

18.

3.

7.

11.

15.

19.

4.

8.

12.

16.

20.