60742347

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    1/22

    Primate Vocal Communication: A Useful Tool forUnderstanding Human Speech and Language Evolution?

    PAWEL FEDUREK1

    AND KATIE E. SLOCOMBE1

    Abstract Language is a uniquely human trait, and questions of how and

    why it evolved have been intriguing scientists for years. Nonhuman primates

    (primates) are our closest living relatives, and their behavior can be used to

    estimate the capacities of our extinct ancestors. As humans and many

    primate species rely on vocalizations as their primary mode of communica-

    tion, the vocal behavior of primates has been an obvious target for studies

    investigating the evolutionary roots of human speech and language. By

    studying the similarities and differences between human and primate

    vocalizations, comparative research has the potential to clarify the evolu-

    tionary processes that shaped human speech and language. This review

    examines some of the seminal and recent studies that contribute to our

    knowledge regarding the link between primate calls and human language and

    speech. We focus on three main aspects of primate vocal behavior:

    functional reference, call combinations, and vocal learning. Studies in these

    areas indicate that despite important differences, primate vocal communica-

    tion exhibits some key features characterizing human language. They alsoindicate, however, that some critical aspects of speech, such as vocal

    plasticity, are not shared with our primate cousins. We conclude that

    comparative research on primate vocal behavior is a very promising tool for

    deepening our understanding of the evolution of human speech and lan-

    guage, but much is still to be done as many aspects of monkey and ape

    vocalizations remain largely unexplored.

    Tools for Tackling Language and Speech Evolution

    Language is an exceedingly complex and intricate behavior and is one ofthe capacities that appears to distinguish humans from the rest of the living

    world. Language enables humans to represent and communicate complex

    abstract information, and it occurs in verbal, gestural, and written forms. An

    evolutionary account for this capacity remains elusive, but this issue is the focus

    of interdisciplinary research effort. One premise is that many of the cognitive

    capacities involved in language processing are much older than language itself,

    1

    Department of Psychology, University of York, YO10 5DD York, United Kingdom.

    Human Biology, April 2011, v. 83, no. 2, pp. 153173.

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    2/22

    with their phylogenetic roots deep in the primate lineage (Hauser et al. 2002).

    Thus, a promising empirical approach to understanding the origins of language

    is to apply a comparative approach (Hauser et al. 2002). In particular, by

    examining the capacities of extant primates, whose phylogenetic relationships to

    modern humans are known, we can draw inferences about the likely capacities of

    our extinct ancestors. Specifically, by investigating primate behavior it is

    possible to test the hypothesis that human language built on abilities already

    present in the primate lineage before the advent of modern humans. This

    comparative approach is vital for identifying the homologous, shared elements of

    language which appear to have evolved gradually from a common primate

    ancestor and those which have no clear evolutionary path and thus may be the

    novel elements which were instrumental to human language evolving into its

    current uniquely complex state (Hauser et al. 2002). In addition, comparative

    research can allow researchers to develop and test hypotheses concerning the

    evolutionary pressures that may have driven the selection of traits required for

    language.

    Whilst the capacity for language is independent of modality of production

    and perception, speech is the pervasive and most prevalent way of conveying

    language in modern humans. Speech, which relies on the vocalauditory channel,

    is characterized by voluntary control and is a culturally shared system for

    communicating within a population. Speech is also combinatorial, with pho-

    nemes and syllables being combined to form words and phrases. Humans seem

    to have several adaptations for speech production and perception, including those

    related to the structure of the vocal tract and neural mechanisms in the brain

    (Fitch 2000; Pinker 1994). Understanding how these adaptations evolved is

    central to our understanding of how language evolved into its current speech-

    dominated form. Although no other primate is equipped with speech apparatus as

    sophisticated as the one found in humans, comparative data from nonhuman

    primates have been central to furthering our understanding of speech evolution

    (Fitch 2000; Ghazanfar and Hauser 1999; Riede et al. 2005).

    Why Vocalizations?

    To investigate the evolution of human speech from a comparative

    perspective it is necessary to focus on the vocalauditory channel of communi-

    cation in other species, including primates (Ghazanfar and Hauser 1999). As the

    capacity for language is independent of modality, research into any modality of

    communication in other species could further our understanding of language

    evolution: so what contribution can primate vocalizations make?

    Vocalizations are an important mode of communication for most primates,as in general they are well suited to both their social and physical environments.

    i i li i b l l i ibili i d i di id

    154 / FEDUREK AND SLOCOMBE

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    3/22

    not be visible. Empirically more research has been conducted on vocal behavior

    than any other type of communication in primates (Slocombe et al. in press) and

    researchers are well equipped to conduct meaningful research into vocalizations

    and their cognitive underpinnings because of the techniques and methods that can

    be applied to vocal behavior. Detailed acoustic analysis of calls in combination

    with observational data allow us to examine the production of calls in great

    detail, and experimental playback techniques crucially allow us to create and test

    precise hypotheses regarding listener understanding of vocal signals. In contrast

    to other modalities, both observational and experimental vocal techniques can be

    conducted in the wild, meaning that many primate vocal communication studies

    have high ecological validity.

    It is important to highlight that facial expressions, body postures, manual

    gestures, and olfaction also play important roles in primate communication and

    communicative signals are often composites of two or more of these different

    modalities. In this way focusing exclusively on vocal communication is telling

    only part of the story, but unfortunately it is out of the scope of this review to

    cover the other modalities and multimodal signals and how they also make a

    valuable contribution to our understanding of language evolution.

    Comparative data on primate gestural and vocal communication are often

    used to argue whether the origin of human language was vocal or gestural (Arbib

    2008; Hewes 1973; Riede et al. 2005; Tomasello 2008; Zuberbuhler 2005). Both

    modalities demonstrate some aspects of continuity with language, with great ape

    gestures being learnt, generative and intentional signals (Tomasello 2008) and

    monkey vocalizations functioning referentially and being combined into se-

    quences (Zuberbuhler 2005). Although data on vocal and gestural competencies

    are important for furthering this debate, currently we do not have comparable

    data in the two modalities to make very meaningful comparisons (Slocombe et al.

    in press). For example most gestural data are collected from great apes, whereas

    most vocal data are collected from monkeys: any conclusions of the relative

    merits of one modality over the other are confounded by systematic differences

    in the study species, study environment (wild/captive), and focus of the research.

    Although vocalizations are central to arguments over the origin of language and this

    is another example of how primate vocalizations can make a strong contribution to

    debates surrounding language evolution, it is not the focus of this review.

    Precursors to Human Language and Speech?

    Primate vocalizations can be useful tools in examining many different

    questions within the language evolution debate, including the evolutionary

    pressures that may have led to more complex communication and the modalityin which human language arose (vocal vs. gestural). They can also help us

    id if d di i i h b f f h d l hi h

    Primate Vocal Communication / 155

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    4/22

    manner, but we focus on two key cognitive components of language where there

    has been substantial and important evidence collected from primates: we will

    examine evidence for potential precursors to human reference and basic

    combinatorial rules. In addition, we will examine vocal learning to illustrate how

    primate vocalizations can contribute to our understanding of speech evolution

    and how discontinuities between primates and humans are just as valuable in

    developing our understanding as evidence for continuities. It is important to note

    here that the aim of this paper is to provide a review of seminal and recent

    literature in relation to the topics outlined above, to illustrate the value and

    relevance of primate vocalizations to the debate surrounding language evolution,

    rather than to offer new insights or hypotheses regarding the reviewed topic.

    Given that recent works have questioned the utility of primate vocalizations for

    furthering our understanding of language evolution (e.g., Arbib 2008; Tomasello

    2008), we believe this review is important for highlighting the contribution

    primate vocal behavior can make to the interdisciplinary research effort focused

    on language evolution.

    Functional Reference

    Human language is highly efficient at conveying meaningful messages

    between communicators. Semantics plays a crucial role in this, as human words,

    phrases, and sentences all carry meaning. Within the field of semantics, many

    human words and phrases are referential, in that they refer to an external object

    or event in the world. The specificity of reference varies greatly in human

    language from utterances with many possible referents (mammal) to highly

    specific utterances (my dog). Comparative researchers have identified a process

    within animal vocal communication that shares some of the features of this

    human system, and may represent an important precursor to human linguistic

    abilities. In animals this ability to produce calls that function to convey a

    message to conspecifics about an object or event in the external world has been

    termed functionally referential communication.

    Evidence for Functional Reference in Primates. The seminal study of

    functional reference examined the alarm calls of vervet monkeys. Strushaker

    (1967) first reported that vervet monkeys produce three acoustically distinct

    alarm calls when encountering with their three main predators: leopards, eagles,

    and snakes. Seyfarth et al. (1980) then conducted a playback experiment to

    examine whether these calls were meaningful to listeners, in the sense that these

    three calls represented different classes of predators. The results of the play-back

    experiment demonstrated that listeners responded in exactly the same, adaptivemanner to playbacks of conspecific alarm calls as they did when they encoun-

    d h l d f h l hi h l ll l

    156 / FEDUREK AND SLOCOMBE

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    5/22

    repertoire and produced consistently in response to a specific external stimulus.

    Second, from the receivers perspective, the receiver must react to the call in the

    same way as it would react toward the stimulus itself, in the absence of any other

    information indicating the presence of the stimulus that elicited the call (Marler

    et al. 1992).

    Since the famous study on vervet monkeys, functionally referential alarm

    calls have been found in a number of other primate species, such as Diana

    monkeys (Zuberbuhler et al. 1997), Campbell monkeys (Zuberbuhler 2001),

    putty-nosed monkeys (Arnold and Zuberbuhler 2006b), tamarins (Kirchhof and

    Hammerschmidt 2006), and ring-tailed lemurs (Macedonia 1990). The specific-

    ity of the alarm calling system varies, with some systems that seem to convey

    information about the presence of an aerial or terrestrial threat (Macedonia and

    Stanger 1994; Oda and Masataka 1996) and other systems being specific to types

    of predator, regardless of the location of the predator. For instance the playback

    of predator stimuli from different elevations and distances confirmed that

    regardless of the urgency to respond or the trajectory of an imminent attack Diana

    monkeys did not deviate from labeling the type of predator with their alarm calls

    (Zuberbuhler 2000b). It has been suggested that the specificity of alarm calls

    in these animals results from differences in the nature of escape responses

    that are required to avoid different types of predatory threat (Macedonia

    1990). Selection pressures seem to have favored the evolution of acousticallyspecific calls that generate specific responses in the receivers, if differential

    anti-predator responses increase fitness (Donaldson et al. 2007; Macedonia

    1990; Pereira and Macedonia 1991).

    It is not only in urgent predator avoidance contexts that functional

    reference has been found. Primates also produce functionally referential calls in

    response to food. Again there is variation in the specificity of these calls, with

    tufted capuchin monkeys producing calls that simply alert listeners to the

    presence of food in the environment (Di Bitetti 1993) and other species

    producing vocalizations that seem to provide listeners with information about thenature of the discovered food source. Rhesus macaques give different calls to

    high and low value food items (Hauser and Marler 1993) and a habituation-

    dishabituation experiment showed that listeners behaved as if they extracted

    meaning about the value of the food source from these calls (Hauser 1998). More

    recently chimpanzees have been shown to produce acoustically distinct calls to

    foods of different values, and in captivity this system even extends to specific call

    variants being produced in response to different high value food items (bread,

    mango, banana) that remain stable over feeding events (Slocombe and Zuber-

    buhler 2006). A playback study showed that listeners can extract informationabout quality of food from the calls of group members (Slocombe and

    Z b b hl 2005b) b h h h f d ifi ll ( b d )

    Primate Vocal Communication / 157

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    6/22

    1993], cotton-top tamarins [Roush and Snowdon 2000], white-faced capuchins

    [Gros-Louis 2003], bonobos [Clay and Zuberbuhler 2009], marmosets [Kitzmann

    and Caine 2009]) or quantity (red bellied tamarins [Caine et al. 1995]). Such

    context-specific calling is necessary for a functionally referential system, but

    listener understanding (i.e., whether listeners act as if they extract information

    about the external event from the call) must be systematically tested before calls

    can be shown to function referentially.

    The complexity of social relationships within primate groups suggests that

    functional reference may also occur in a variety of other social situations. In

    social situations distinct behavioral responses to different contexts are rare and

    the more subtle responses of listeners are often measured by differences in

    looking duration / latency to look at the speaker. Although this shows the

    primates distinguish between the calls, it is harder to infer what meaning they

    extracted from it.

    In the social sphere, female Barbary macaques produce acoustically

    different calls during copulations depending on whether the male has ejaculated

    or not (Pfefferle et al. 2008a). The facts that the call begins well before

    ejaculation and that the frequencies of call production correlate with ejaculation

    rates suggests that these calls are signals directed to the mating partner so as to

    increase the probability of ejaculation (Pfefferle et al. 2008a). Recent playbacks

    have shown that listening males can distinguish between these calls and adjusttheir behavior toward the female (such as walking toward the female or the time

    spent in her vicinity) according to whether she has just had a successful mating

    with another male (Pfefferle et al. 2008b). It is therefore possible that these calls

    also function to provide male listeners with information about whether the

    female is likely to conceive to increase the likelihood of subsequent mating

    (Pfefferle et al. 2008b).

    Primates often vocalize whilst involved in agonistic interactions, and

    rhesus macaque monkeys give different screams in response to differing levels of

    aggression from opponents of different ranks. A playback experiment showedthat mothers were able to meaningfully distinguish (i.e., they responded to these

    calls as if they extracted some information about the ongoing fight from these

    vocalizations) between scream variants given by offspring (Gouzoules et al.

    1984). More recently chimpanzees have been shown to produce acoustically

    distinct screams depending on their social role in the interaction (victim or

    aggressor; [Slocombe and Zuberbuhler 2005a]). A playback study has shown that

    listeners were able to infer the respective roles and the subsequent direction of

    aggression between two screaming individuals from these calls (Slocombe et al.

    2010a). Listeners were thus able to distinguish between sequences of calls thatsimulated agonistic interactions which were either incongruous or congruous

    i h h i i i l d i hi h i h bj h i i ifi l

    158 / FEDUREK AND SLOCOMBE

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    7/22

    conducted in the wild demonstrated that listeners distinguished between these

    screams (Slocombe et al. 2009). Listeners showed significantly more interest in

    screams given in response to severe aggression than mild aggression. These

    responses were not driven by low level responses to the acoustic structure of the

    different screams, as listeners did not show comparable interest in control

    tantrum screams, which matched the severe screams in acoustic structure.

    Evidence for Functional Reference in Non-Primate Species. Functional

    reference has also been demonstrated in a wide variety of nonprimate species.

    For instance, chickens produce functionally referential food calls (Evans and

    Marler 1994) and alarm calls that function to refer to aerial and terrestrial

    predatory threats (Evans et al. 1993). Meerkats also produce alarm calls that not

    only function to denote predator type but also the urgency of response (Manser

    2001; Manser et al. 2002). Although the surface behavior of these diverse species

    is similar, there is some evidence that primates seem to process these calls in a

    relatively sophisticated way. For instance, experiments with wild Diana monkeys

    indicated that listeners attended to the meaning of the call rather than the surface

    acoustics and likely form some kind of mental representation when hearing a call

    (Zuberbuhler et al. 1999). This study showed that upon hearing a sound

    indicating the presence of a specific predator, such as leopard growls, Diana

    monkeys respond much less intensively if they had been primed by a conspe-cifics leopard alarm call (Zuberbuhler et al. 1999). The leopard growls and the

    leopard alarm call are two acoustically different sounds, suggesting that the

    monkeys were habituating not to the low level acoustic structure of the stimuli

    but to the referent of the sounds. Unfortunately, comparable experiments

    probing the mechanisms underlying functionally referential signals in nonpri-

    mates have not been reported, so it is difficult to conclude how the presence of

    similar behavior in nonprimate species should affect our interpretation of the data

    in relation to humans. It is likely, however, that similar evolutionary pressures

    have lead to the emergence of this kind of signaling in divergent species. Furtherinvestigation of functional reference in other nonprimate species may further

    shed light on the selection pressures that favor its emergence and clarify

    similarities and differences in the psychological underpinnings of this behavior in

    different species.

    Human Language and Functional Reference. The studies of functionally

    referential calls indicate some continuity between primate and human commu-

    nication. However, it is important to highlight some crucial differences between

    functional reference in animals and reference in humans. First, the referentialcharacter of human language is arbitrary in the sense that humans flexibly attach

    diff i diff b l ( d ) h h i

    Primate Vocal Communication / 159

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    8/22

    mechanisms underlying communication are similar. This is partly because of a

    paucity of research tools we have available to examine the cognitive machinery

    involved in nonhuman animal vocal communication (Cheney and Seyfarth 1990).

    As outlined above, there is some evidence that some primate listeners may form

    some kind of mental representation when hearing functionally referential calls

    (e.g., Zuberbuhler et al. 1999). However, it is very difficult to examine the nature

    of any mental representation held by a primate. For instance, when a vervet

    monkey responds appropriately to a leopard alarm call, it is impossible to tell

    whether the calls evoke a declarative representation of a leopard in the mind of

    the listener or whether they interpret the call as an imperative such as climb the

    tree. Although we sometimes use such metaphors as information, message,

    and meaningful, we might never be sure exactly what happens in the listeners

    mind. Furthermore, the involvement of such mental representations may not be

    required to produce and respond appropriately to referential signals, and more

    simple mechanisms may account for these effects (Hauser et al. 2002; Owren and

    Rendall 2001; Seyfarth et al. 2010). Parsimony is strength in science for a good

    reason: simple mechanisms are more likely to evolve, and therefore we should

    first explore the simplest possible explanations for any behavior, including

    functionally referential calls, before reaching for more elaborated ones. It is for

    these reasons that the term functionally referential communication refers to the

    way signals are used and the way responses are generated, but not in any senseto the cognitive mechanisms that are involved (Marler et al. 1992; Owren and

    Rendall 2001).

    The cognitive mechanisms involved in primate vocal behavior remains a

    contentious issue and a topic of considerable recent debate. Rendall et al. (2009),

    for example, challenged the view that the concept of information is needed to

    explain any aspect of vocal communication in nonhuman animals. Their key

    argument is that the mechanisms involved in primate communication differ from

    those characterizing human language mainly as a result of the cognitive

    limitation of these animals. The authors claim that nonhuman animals are notcapable of intentional communication and perspective-taking and consequently

    advocate abandoning any language-like expressions such as information

    when dealing with primate vocalizations. In contrast, Seyfarth et al. (2010) claim

    that the term information applies simply to the process of reducing uncertainty

    by learning associations between one stimulus (such as a vocal signal) and

    another (such as the presence of a predator), rather than to the elaborate process

    of encoding and decoding of ideas that occurs in human language. The process

    of making associations between calls and objects or events in external world is

    favored by natural selection as it allows animals predict accurately critical eventsin the environment. The fact that this process is rather simple has prompted some

    h h h i f i f f i d

    160 / FEDUREK AND SLOCOMBE

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    9/22

    The role of receivers in animal communication has also been extensively

    discussed. Rendall et al. (2009) suggest that primate vocalizations function to

    manipulate the behaviors of receivers through the direct induction of their

    nervous systems. Rendall et al. (2009) along with Owren et al. (in press)

    characterize listeners as passive receivers and reject the notion that receivers

    extract and infer information from calls in any way similar to human language

    processing. Seyfarth et al. (2010) criticize this approach, suggesting that it

    focuses too heavily on the interests of senders while neglecting the interests of

    receivers in animal communication. The evolution of communicative signals

    requires the interplay of signalers and receivers, as signals can be ignored by the

    receivers if they are costly or fail to provide benefits (Searcy and Nowicki 2005;

    Zahavi and Zahavi 1997). Therefore, the receivers are not prisoners of the

    manipulative tactics used by the senders (Seyfarth et al. 2010) and rather play an

    active role in communication and the evolution of signals.

    Mechanisms Underlying Call Production. Although there are many areas of

    disagreement regarding animal communication, there does seem to be a general

    agreement that the motivations and mechanisms that drive call production in

    primates likely differ from those involved in human language. Human language

    is generally characterized by the producer intending to convey a message to the

    receiver, but to date we have no evidence to suggest that such intentionalcommunication exists in monkeys. Indeed, in the case of baboon contact barks,

    although calls are exchanged between individuals, it was found that call

    production was contingent on the callers themselves being at the periphery of the

    group, rather than a reflection of a motivation to provide an informative reply to

    other lost individuals (Cheney et al. 1996). It is argued that the ability to

    distinguish between others and own knowledge is a key prerequisite for such

    informative and intentional communication (Grice 1957), and given the generally

    poor performance of monkey species on theory of mind (i.e., the ability to

    attribute mental stages to others) tests (Heyes 1998; Povinelli et al. 1991), thistype of communication may be impossible. This view is supported by a study in

    which rhesus monkey mothers were given the opportunity to inform ignorant

    offspring about food, or a danger in the environment, and they failed to do so

    (Cheney and Seyfarth 1990).

    Great apes, in contrast to monkeys, produce manual gestures that show

    some hallmarks of intentionality (Leavens 2004) such as sensitivity to the visual

    attention of the recipient (Liebal et al. 2004) and persistence and elaboration of

    the signal if the goal of the signaler is not achieved (Leavens et al. 2005). Recent

    evidence also indicates that great apes may have a superior level of theory ofmind skills, and when they are tested in competitive paradigms they seem to be

    bl di i i h h h i di id l d k (H l 2001 H

    Primate Vocal Communication / 161

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    10/22

    others (Call and Tomasello 1999). Great apes failed, however, to show any

    understanding of false belief (Kaminski et al. 2008; Krachun et al. 2009), which

    is seen as the pinnacle of a fully fledged set of theory of mind skills that in

    humans typically develops at the age of four (de Villiers and Pyers 2002). The

    ability to understand beliefs of others is argued to have underpinned the evolution

    of complex social cognition phenomena in humans, including language, which

    relies on a set of communally shared symbols with arbitrary assigned meanings

    (Tomasello et al. 2005; Tomasello and Carpenter 2007; Fitch et al. 2010). In

    summary, although there are critical differences in theory of mind skills

    demonstrated by apes and humans, it seems that great apes can assign some

    mental states to others. The extent to which these skills are put to pragmatic use

    in vocal communication is still to be tested. Research is currently underway to

    examine whether the differences between monkeys and apes in terms of theory

    of mind skills and gesture use result in great ape species engaging in more

    informative, intentional vocal communication than monkeys (K. Slocombe,

    unpublished data). Until evidence is forthcoming, however, it is most parsimo-

    nious to assume that the cognitive mechanisms involved in ape vocalizations are

    similar to those in the better-studied monkey species.

    Whilst it is unclear what mechanisms and motivations underlie primate call

    production, several lines of evidence indicate that calls are not necessarily rigid

    and automatic responses. Call producers seem sensitive to the presence of anaudience and for instance, vervet monkeys rarely produce alarm calls when alone

    (Cheney and Seyfarth 1990).

    In addition, the composition of the audience also affects call production

    both in terms of the frequency of calling (Caine et al. 1995; Chapman and

    Lefebvre 1990; Di Bitetti 2005; Roush and Snowdon 2000; Slocombe et al.

    2010b) and the acoustic structure of the call production (Slocombe and

    Zuberbuhler 2007). Finally, there is some evidence that call producers are

    sensitive to the behavior of others, with male langur monkeys continuing to alarm

    call until all members of the group call (Wich and de Vries 2006) and male bluemonkeys taking into account the danger faced by group members during calling

    in response to eagle predation (Papworth et al. 2008).

    Primate call production thus seems sensitive to subtle social variables,

    indicating that social complexity may have been an important pressure in

    language evolution. As with functional reference little is known, however, about

    the cognitive mechanisms that underpin audience effects of call production

    (Zuberbuhler 2008). The fact that fish and domestic chickens adjust general

    social behavior according to the audience composition (Grosenick et al. 2007;

    Herb et al. 2003; Marler et al. 1986) means that complex cognitive tools may notbe required for audience effects to take place: it might just be that the presence

    f i i di id l i l i l h i ll i i

    162 / FEDUREK AND SLOCOMBE

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    11/22

    that the psychological mechanisms involved in audience effect in primates might

    be more complex that in other animals (Zuberbuhler 2008). More research is,

    however, needed to explore in detail the cognitive underpinnings of audience

    effects in primates before such conclusions can be reached.

    Summary of Functional Reference. The study of functional reference in

    primates provides several fruitful avenues for language evolution research.

    Although there are key differences between human reference and functional

    reference, the study of functionally referential signals allows us to assess the

    type, complexity, and specificity of information that can be communicated

    between conspecifics. It has been particularly useful in highlighting the flexibil-

    ity of the listener in being able to respond appropriately to a wide variety of

    signals (Cheney and Seyfarth 2005), which may shed light on how our own

    comprehension skills evolved. In parallel with a broader comparative approach,

    we can assess the environments and selective pressures that give rise to

    communication of highly specific messages. The current body of work on

    functional reference also lays vital ground work for further investigations which

    may be particularly pertinent to language evolution. For instance researchers

    must establish the meaning of single call types before they can examine how and

    why primates may combine their calls, which may provide an insight into

    precursors for human syntax.

    Vocal Plasticity and Vocal Learning

    Humans are able to imitate a wide range of noises: the ability relies on the

    engagement of the larynx to generate acoustic variation and to produce novel

    vocalizations. Such vocal plasticity is a key property of human speech (Fitch

    2000). Comparative research has revealed such vocal plasticity seems to be

    shared to some degree with song birds, parrots, dolphins, and seals (Janik and

    Slater 1997). Recent studies have shown that chimpanzees in captivity use a

    novel raspberry sound as an attention-getter (Hopkins et al. 2007) and anorang-utan has spontaneously learnt to whistle (Wich et al. 2009), but these

    sound innovations crucially do not engage the larynx, which is vital for speech

    production (Fitch 2000). In contrast, there is no good evidence for plasticity in

    primate vocalizations (which rely on laryngeal activity). Despite extensive

    training, all attempts to teach a human-raised chimpanzee to speak even a few

    words failed (Hayes 1951). There is a considerable body of evidence that shows

    that, in contrast to humans, the vocal repertoire of primates is fixed and genetically

    determined. For example, from the first days of life a squirrel monkey is able to

    produce acoustically adult-like vocalizations (Winter 1969). Early deafness does notstop a squirrel monkey from producing the full vocal repertoire typical of their

    i i l ddi i k i f i d i h h ifi

    Primate Vocal Communication / 163

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    12/22

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    13/22

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    14/22

    encountered by other animals, which seem to have shaped their vocal behavior

    in specific ways (Macedonia 1990). However, in contrast to the majority of

    animals, most primate species also live in social groups that are characterized by

    unusually complex network of relationships with kin and non-kin, where more

    sophisticated communication could give a fitness advantage. Two mechanisms

    that would allow primates to increase the number of messages they can communicate

    working with a fixed repertoire, which are highly relevant to speech and language

    evolution, are categorical perception and call combinations.

    On a phonetic level, there can be substantial grading between human

    phonemes (e.g., graded continuum between ba and pa), yet humans perceive

    these graded signals categorically (Burling 1993; Rosner and Pickering 1994).

    Many primates, and particularly apes, have highly graded vocal repertoires, and

    there is some evidence that some monkey species also perceive graded calls

    categorically (May et al. 1989; Fischer 1998; Fischer et al. 2000; Fisher et al.

    2001). Indeed, nonhuman animals have also been shown to perceive continuums

    from human speech categorically (e.g., Kuhl and Miller 1975), indicating that

    auditory categorical perception is likely to occur in a number of species. More

    systematic research is needed to reveal how different primate species perceive

    conspecific calls, as currently we may be underestimating the size of their vocal

    repertoires.

    Categorical perception of graded repertoires may increase the number of

    distinct calls available to convey different messages, but the repertoire is still

    fixed because of the very limited vocal plasticity in primates. There are several

    ways in which this can be overcome. First, primates can use pragmatic strategies,

    where information from calls is combined with world knowledge to increase the

    range of messages that can be conveyed. We have good evidence that primate

    listeners are not simple passive receivers of sound, but they do engage in

    pragmatic reasoning (Zuberbuhler 2000a), combine information from calls with

    knowledge of social relationships (Cheney et al. 1995; Slocombe et al. 2010a),

    and may even perform basic inferential reasoning (Arnold and Zuberbuhler,

    submitted) to respond appropriately to the calls they hear. Second, by combining

    existing calls to create combinations that elicit different responses in listeners

    compared with the component calls, the number of messages that can be

    communicated is increased.

    Call Combinations

    Humans can convey an infinite amount of messages using a limited number

    of words because of the powerful system of grammatical rules that govern the

    structure and form of language, including the ordering of words into meaningfulsentences. Although it seems highly unlikely that any other species has syntactic

    l l h f h i id i h id f

    166 / FEDUREK AND SLOCOMBE

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    15/22

    It is known that primates often produce call combinations composed of two

    or more calls and that these calls are sometimes produced in a nonrandom order

    and in specific contexts (Mitani and Marler 1989; Robinson 1984; Snowdon and

    Cleveland 1984). For instance, experiments showed that black and white colobus

    monkeys produce specific call combinations in response to specific predators

    (Schel et al. 2009). Similarly, experimental studies with white-handed gibbons

    have suggested that recombination of particular notes within songs, which

    normally function to strengthen pair-bonding and to attract mates, allows these

    primates to communicate about predator-induced danger (Clarke et al. 2006).

    Evidence for one call acting as a modifier for another has been provided by

    systematic studies of Diana monkey comprehension of Campbell monkey alarm calls

    (Zuberbuhler 2002). Campbell monkeys produce distinct alarm calls to both leopards

    and eagles, but in addition they also produce a boom call as a general alert call.

    Campbell monkeys sometimes produce booms followed by predator specific alarm

    calls, and the addition of the boom call seems to modify the meaning of the

    subsequent alarm call. For example, in a playback experiment Zuberbuhler showed

    that while Diana monkeys typically produce their own leopard call upon hearing

    Campbells monkey leopard calls, the Diana monkeys do not produce alarm calls

    upon hearing Campbells monkey boom call followed by the leopard call

    (Zuberbuhler 2002). This study indicates that the boom call modifies the functional

    meaning of the subsequent leopard alarm call.Male putty-nosed monkeys produce sequences of two loud calls that function

    to convey at least three different messages to listeners (Arnold and Zuberbuhler

    2006a; Arnold and Zuberbuhler 2006b). Sequences of pyows are given to a broad

    range of disturbances, including leopards, and sequences of hacks are generally given

    in response to eagles. The male monkeys also produce Pyow-hack sequences,

    which initiate group travel. Rigorous playback experiments demonstrated that the

    lets go message conveyed by the Pyow-hack sequence was contingent on the

    sequence of the calls, not the acoustic structure of the calls themselves (Arnold and

    Zuberbuhler 2008). This study illustrates how combining calls can enable a greaterflexibility in terms of the functional usage of calls.

    There is more evidence of primates combining calls in potentially complex

    sequences, but the extent to which this enables individuals to communicate different

    or modified messages is unknown because the way listeners respond to these

    combinations has yet to be examined. For example, chimpanzees often combine call

    types in short sequences, and certain call combinations are given in narrowly defined

    contexts (Crockford and Boesch 2005). More recently, potentially high levels of

    complexity within call combinations have been discovered in the systematic study of

    Campbells monkeys. This species produces numerous combinations of calls inresponse to a wide variety of different events, from trees falling to neighbor

    (O l 2009) Fi ll C b ll k f

    Primate Vocal Communication / 167

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    16/22

    shown to be meaningful to listeners, these calling systems will represent an excellent

    example of how combining existing calls in a repertoire can increase the complexity

    of information that can be communicated.

    Conclusion

    In summary, the study of primate communication is a promising avenue to

    explore in several ways. First, primate vocalizations can help establish what the

    phylogenetically old and uniquely human aspects of language and speech may

    have been during the evolutionary process. In particular, this review highlights

    some of the areas of continuity within the vocal domain, where the primate

    ability for functional reference, complex comprehension, and combinations of

    calls may represent precursors to linguistic abilities. This review also highlightsdiscontinuity in terms of vocal plasticity and the comparably scant evidence for

    modification of calls through learning, suggesting that fine vocal control and the

    ability to generate novel calls that are required for speech likely evolved once

    humans split from the rest of the primate lineage. As such, primate communi-

    cation has much to offer the field of language evolution. However, in many

    species we know virtually nothing about their vocal behavior. Much more

    research effort is required to exploit this potentially fruitful way of exploring how

    and why human language may have evolved.

    Received 1 April 2010; revision accepted for publication 20 September 2010.

    Literature Cited

    Arbib, M. A., K. Liebal, and S. Pika. 2008. Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human

    language. Curr. Anthropol. 49:10521075.

    Arnold, K., and K. Zuberbuhler. 2006a. The alarm-calling system of adult male putty-nosed monkeys,

    Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Anim. Behav.72:643653.

    Arnold, K., and K. Zuberbuhler. 2006b. Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature 441:303.Arnold, K., and K. Zuberbuhler. 2008. Meaningful call combinations in a non-human primate.Curr.

    Biol. 18:R202R203.

    Arnold, K., and K. Zuberbuhler. n.d. Inference enables wild monkeys to determine the cause of alarm

    calls. In review.

    Benz, J. J. 1993. Food-elicited vocalizations in golden lion tamarins: Design features for represen-

    tational communication. Anim. Behav. 45:443455.

    Burling, R. 1993. Primate calls, human language, and nonverbal communication. Curr. Anthropol.

    34:2553.

    Caine, N. G., R. L. Addington, and T. L. Windfelder. 1995. Factors affecting the rates of food calls

    given by red-bellied tamarins. Anim. Behav. 50:5360.

    Call, J., and M. Tomasello. 1999. A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children andgreat apes. Child Dev. 70:381395.

    Call J and M Tomasello 2008 Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later Trends

    168 / FEDUREK AND SLOCOMBE

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    17/22

    Chapman, C. A., and L. Lefebvre. 1990. Manipulating foraging group size: Spider monkey food calls

    at fruiting trees. Anim. Behav. 39:891896.

    Cheney, D., and R. M. Seyfarth. 1990. How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another

    Species. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Cheney, D. L., and R. M. Seyfarth. 2005. Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of

    language evolution. Linguistic Review 22:135159.

    Cheney, D. L., R. M. Seyfarth, and R. Palombit. 1996. The function and mechanisms underlying

    baboon contact barks. Anim. Behav. 52:507518.

    Cheney, D. L., R. M. Seyfarth, and J. B. Silk. 1995. The role of grunts in reconciling opponents and

    facilitating interactions among adult female baboons. Anim. Behav. 50:249257.

    Clark Arcadi, A. 1996. Phrase structure of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: Patterns of production and

    interpopulation variability. Am. J. Primatol. 39:159178.

    Clarke, E., U. H. Reichard, and K. Zuberbuhler. 2006. The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs.

    PLoS One 1:e73.

    Clay, Z., and K. Zuberbuhler. 2009. Food-associated calling sequences in bonobos. Anim. Behav.77:13871396.

    Crockford, C., and C. Boesch. 2005. Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour.

    142:397421.

    Crockford, C., I. Herbinger, L. Vigilant et al. 2004. Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls:

    A case for vocal learning? Ethology. 110:221243.

    Di Bitetti, M. S. 1993. Food-associated calls of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus) are

    functionally referential signals. Behaviour. 140:565592.

    Di Bitetti, M. S. 2005. Food-associated calls and audience effects in tufted capuchin monkeys, Cebus

    apella nigritus. Anim. Behav. 69:911919.

    Donaldson, C. M., M. Lachmanm, and C. T. Bergstrom. 2007. The evolution of functionally

    referential meaning in a structured world. J. Theor. Biol. 246:225233.

    Egnor, S. E., and M. D. Hauser. 2004. A paradox in the evolution of primate vocal learning. Trends

    Neurosci. 27:649654.

    Elowson, A. M., and C. T. Snowdon. 1994. Pygmy marmosets, Cebullea pygmaea, modify vocal

    structure in response to changed social environment. Anim. Behav. 47:12671277.

    Enard, W., S. Gehre, K. Hammerschmidt et al. 2009. A humanized version of Foxp2 affects

    cortico-basal ganglia circuits in mice. Cell. 137:961971.

    Enard, W., M. Przeworski, S. E. Fisher et al. 2002. Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved

    in speech and language. Nature. 418:869872.

    Evans, C. S., L. Evans, and P. Marler. 1993. On the meaning of alarm calls: Functional reference in

    an avian vocal system. Anim. Behav. 46:2338.Evans, C. S., and P. Marler. 1994. Food calling and audience effects in male chickens,Gallus gallus:

    Their relationships to food availability, courtship and social facilitation. Anim. Behav.

    47:11591170.

    Ey, E., C. Rahn, K. Hammerschmidt et al. 2009. Wild female olive baboons adapt their grunt

    vocalizations to environmental conditions. Ethology. 115:493503.

    Fischer, J. 1998. Barbary macaques categorize shrill barks into two call types. Anim. Behav.

    55:799807.

    Fischer, J., D. L. Cheney, and R. M. Seyfarth. 2000. Development of infant baboons responses to

    graded bark variants. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 267:23172321.

    Fischer, J., M. Metz, D. L. Cheney et al. 2001. Baboon responses to graded bark variants. Anim.

    Behav. 61:925931.Fitch, W. T. 2000. The evolution of speech: A comparative review.Trends Cogn. Sci.4(7):258267.

    Fitch W T L Huber and T Bugnyar 2010 Social cognition and the evolution of language:

    Primate Vocal Communication / 169

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    18/22

    Ghazanfar, A. A., and M. D. Hauser. 1999. The neuroethology of primate vocal communication:

    Substrates for the evolution of speech. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3:377384.

    Gouzoules, H., and M. Gouzoules. 1992. Matrilineal signatures in the recruitment screams of pigtail

    macaques, Macaca nemestrina. Behaviour115:327347.Gouzoules, S., H. Gouzoules, and P. Marler. 1984. Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams:

    Representational signalling in the recruitment of agonistic aid. Anim. Behav. 32:182193.

    Grice, H. P. 1957. Meaning. Phil. Rev. 66:377388.

    Grosenick, L., T. S. Clement, and R. D. Fernald. 2007. Fish can infer social rank by observation

    alone. Nature. 445:429 432.

    Gros-Louis, J. 2003. The function of food-associated calls in whitefaced capuchin monkeys, Cebus

    capucinus, from the perspective of the signaller. Anim. Behav. 140:565592.

    Hare, B., J. Call, and M. Tomasello. 2001. Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Anim.

    Behav. 61:139151.

    Hare, B., J. Call, and M. Tomasello. 2006. Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding.

    Cognition 101:495514.Hauser, M. 1992. Articulatory and social factors influence the acoustic structure of rhesus monkey

    vocalizations: A learned mode of production? J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 91:21752179.

    Hauser, M. 1998. Functional referents and acoustic similarity: Field playback experiments with

    rhesus monkeys. Anim. Behav. 55:16471658.

    Hauser, M., N. Chomsky, and W. T. Fitch. 2002. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and

    how did it evolve? Science 298:15691579.

    Hauser, M. D., and P. Marler. 1993. Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulata). 1.

    Socioecological factors. Behav. Ecol. 4:194205.

    Hayes, C. 1951. The ape in our house. Oxford, U.K.: Harper.

    Herb, B. M., S. A. Biron, and M. R. Kidd. 2003. Courtship by subordinate male Siamese fighting fish,

    Betta splendens: Their response to eavesdropping and naive females. Behaviour140:7178.

    Hewes, G. W. 1973. Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Curr. Anthropol.

    12:524.

    Heyes, C. M. 1998. Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behav. Brain. Sci. 21:101104.

    Hopkins, W. D., J. Taglialatela, and D. A. Leavens. 2007. Chimpanzees differentially produce novel

    vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Anim. Behav. 73:281286.

    Janik, V. M., and P. J. B. Slater. 1997. Vocal learning in mammals. Advances in Study of Behaviour

    26:5999.

    Kaminski, J., J. Call, and M. Tomasello. 2008. Chimpanzees know what others have seen but not

    what others believe. Folia primatologica 79:344345.

    Kirchhof, J., and K. Hammerschmidt. 2006. Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins(Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax)Evidence from playback experiments.

    Ethology 112:346354.

    Kitzmann, C. D., and N. G. Caine. 2009. Marmoset (Callithrix geoffroyi) food-associated calls are

    functionally referential. Ethology 115:439 448.

    Krachun, C., M. Carpenter, J. Call et al. 2009. A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children

    and apes. Dev. Sci. 12:521535.

    Kuhl, P.K., and J. D. Miller. 1975. Speech perception by the chinchilla: Voiced-voiceless distinction

    in alveolar plosive consonants. Science 190:69.

    Leavens, D. A. 2004. Manual deixis in apes and humans. Interaction Studies 5(3):387408.

    Leavens, D. A., J. L. Russell, and W. D. Hopkins. 2005. Intentionality as measured in the persistence

    and elaboration of communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).Child Dev.76:291306.Liebal, K., S. Pika, J. Call et al. 2004. To move or not to move: How apes adjust to the attentional

    state of others Interaction Studies 5:199 219

    170 / FEDUREK AND SLOCOMBE

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    19/22

    Manser, M. B. 2001. The acoustic structure of suricates alarm calls varies with predator type and the

    level of response urgency. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 268:23152324.

    Manser, M. B., R. M. Seyfarth, and D. L. Cheney. 2002. Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and

    urgency. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6:5557.Marler, P., A. Dufty, and R. Pickert. 1986. Vocal communication in the domestic chickens. 2. Is a

    sender sensitive to the presence and nature of a receiver? Anim. Behav. 34:194198.

    Marler, P., C. S. Evans, and M. D. Hauser. 1992. Animal signals: Motivational, referential, or both?

    In Nonverbal Vocal Communication: Comparative and Developmental Approaches, H.

    Papousek, U. Jurgens, and M. Papousek, eds. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.

    Marshall, A. J., R. W. Wrangham, and A. C. Arcadi. 1999. Does learning affect the structure of

    vocalizations in chimpanzees? Anim. Behav. 58:825830.

    May, B., D. B. Moody, and W. C. Stebbins. 1989. Categorical perception of conspecific

    communication sounds by Japanese macaques, Macaca fuscata. J. Acoust. Soc. Am.

    85:837847.

    Mitani, J. C., and J. Gros-Louis. 1998. Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: Tests of threehypotheses.Behaviour135:10411064.

    Mitani, J. C., T. Hasegawa, J. Gros-Louis et al. 1992. Dialects in wild chimpanzees. Am. J. Primatol.

    27:233243.A.

    Mitani, J. C., K. L. Hunley, and M. E. Murdoch. 1999. Geographic variation in the calls of wild

    chimpanzees: A reassessment. Am. J. Primatol. 47:133151.

    Mitani, J. C., and P. Marler. 1989. A phonological analysis of male gibbon singing behaviour.

    Behaviour109:2045.

    Nettle, D. 1999. Linguistic diversity. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Oda, R., and N. Masataka. 1996. Interspecific responses of ringtailed lemurs to playback of

    antipredator alarm calls given by Verreauxs sifakas. Ethology 102:441453.

    Ouattara, K., A. Lemasson, and K. Zuberbuhler. 2009. Campbells monkeys use affixation to alter

    call meaning. PLoS One. 4:e7808.

    Owren, M. J., J. A. Dieter, R. M. Seyfarth et al. 1992. Food calls produced by adult female rhesus

    (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques, their normally-raised offspring, and

    offspring cross-fostered between species. Behaviour120:218231.

    Owren, M. J., and D. Rendall. 2001. Sound on the rebound: Bringing form and function back to the

    forefront in understanding nonhuman primate vocal signalling. Evol. Anthropol. 10:5871.

    Owren, M. J., D. Rendall, and M. J. Ryan. 2010. Redefining animal signaling: Influence versus

    information in communication. Biol. Philos. 25:755780.

    Papworth, S., A. S. Bose, J. Barker et al. 2008. Male blue monkeys alarm call in response to danger

    experienced by others. Biol. Lett. 4:472475.Penn, D. C., and D. J. Povinelli. 2007. On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess

    anything remotely resembling a theory of mind. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci.

    362:731744.

    Pereira, M. E., and J. M. Macedonia. 1991. Ringtailed lemur anti-predator calls denote predator class,

    not response urgency. Anim. Behav. 26:760777.

    Pfefferle, D., K. Brauch, M. Heistermann et al. 2008. Female Barbary macaque (Macaca sylvanus)

    copulation calls do not reveal the fertile phase but influence mating outcome. Proc. R. Soc.

    Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 275:571578.

    Pfefferle, D., M. Heistermann, J. K. Hodges et al. 2008. Male Barbary macaques eavesdrop on mating

    outcome: A playback study. Anim. Behav. 75:18851891.

    Pinker, S. 1994: The Language Instinct. New York, NY: HaperCollins.Povinelli, D. J., K. A. Parks, and M. A. Novak. 1991. Do rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) attribute

    knowledge and ignorance to others? J Comp Psychol 105:318 325

    Primate Vocal Communication / 171

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    20/22

    Riede, T., E. Bronson, H. Hatzikirou et al. 2005. Vocal production mechanisms in a non-human

    primate: Morphological data and a model. J. Hum. Evol. 48:8596.

    Robinson, J. G. 1984. Syntactic structures in the vocalisations of wedge-capped capuchin monkeys

    (Cebus nigrivittatus). Behaviour90:4679.Rosner, B. S., and J. B. Pickering. 1994. Vowel Perception and Production. Oxford, U.K.: Oxford

    University Press.

    Roush, R. S., and C. T. Snowdon. 2000. Quality, quantity, distribution and audience effects on food

    calling in cotton-top tamarins. Ethology. 106:673690.

    Schel, A., S. Tranquilli, and K. Zuberbuhler. 2009. The alarm call system of two species of

    black-and-white colobus monkeys (Colobus polykomos and Colobus guereza). J. Comp.

    Psychol.123:136150.

    Searcy, W. A., and S. Nowicki. 2005. The Evolution of Animal Communication: Reliability and

    Deception in Signaling Systems (Monographs in behavior and ecology). Princeton, NJ:

    Princeton University Press.

    Seyfarth, R. M., and D. L. Cheney. 1986. Vocal development in vervet monkeys. Anim. Behav.34:16401658.

    Seyfarth, R. M., D. L. Cheney, T. Bergman et al. 2010. The central importance of information in

    studies of animal communication. Anim. Behav. 80:38.

    Seyfarth, R. M., D. L. Cheney, and P. Marler. 1980. Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic

    communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim. Behav. 28:10701094.

    Slocombe, K. E., S. W. Townsend, and K. Zuberbuhler. 2009. Wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes

    schweinfurthii) distinguish between different scream types: Evidence from a playback study.

    Anim. Cogn. 12:441449.

    Slocombe K. E., B. Waller, and K. Liebal. n.d. The language void: The need for multimodality in

    primate communication research. Anim. Behav. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.02.002. In press.

    Slocombe, K. E., and K. Zuberbuhler. 2007. Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function

    of audience composition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 104:17228 17233.

    Slocombe, K. E., and K. Zuberbuhler. 2005a. Agonistic screams in wild chimpanzees (Pan

    troglodytes schweinfurthii) vary as a function of social role. J. Comp. Psychol. 119:6777.

    Slocombe, K. E., and K. Zuberbuhler. 2005b. Functionally referential communication in a

    chimpanzee. Curr. Biol. 15:17791784.

    Slocombe, K. E., and K. Zuberbuhler. 2006. Food-associated calls in chimpanzees: Responses to food

    types or food preferences? Anim. Behav. 72:989999.

    Slocombe, K. E., T. Kaller, J. Call et al. 2010a. Chimpanzees extract social information from

    agonistic screams. PLoS One 5:e11473.

    Slocombe, K. E., T. Kaller, L. Turman et al. 2010b. Production of food-associated calls in wild malechimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

    64:19591966.

    Snowdon, C. T., and J. Cleveland. 1984. Conversations among pygmy marmosets. Am. J. Primatol.

    7:1520.

    Snowdon, C. T., and A. M. Elowson. 1999. Pygmy marmosets modify call structure when paired.

    Ethology105:893908.

    Struhsaker, T. 1967. Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In

    Social Communication Among Primates, S. A. Altmann, ed. Chicago, IL: University of

    Chicago Press; 281324.

    Sugiura, H. 1993. Temporal and acoustic correlates in vocal exchange of coo calls in Japanese

    macaques. Behaviour124:207225.Sugiura, H. 1998. Matching of acoustic features during the vocal exchange of coo calls by Japanese

    macaques Anim Behav 55:673 687

    172 / FEDUREK AND SLOCOMBE

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    21/22

    Vargha-Khadem, F., D. G. Gadian, A. Copp et al. 2005. FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and

    language. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6:131138.

    de Villiers, J. G., and J. E. Pyers. 2002. Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the

    relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cogn. Dev. 17:10371060.

    Webb, D. M., and J. Zhang. 2005. FoxP2 in song-learning birds and vocal-learning mammals.

    J. Hered.96:212216.

    Wich, S. A., and H. de Vries. 2006. Male monkeys remember which group members have given

    alarm calls. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 273:735740.

    Wich, S. A., K. B. Swartz, M. E. Hardus et al. 2009. A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human

    sound by an orangutan. Primates 50:5664.

    Winter, P. 1969. The variability of peep and twit calls in captive squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus).

    Folia primatologica. 10:204215.

    Winter, P., P. Handley, D. Ploog et al. 1973. Ontogeny of squirrel monkey calls under normal

    conditions and under acoustic isolation. Behaviour47:230239.Zahavi, A., and A. Zahavi. 1997. The Handicap Principle: A Missing Piece of Darwins Puzzle.

    Oxford, U.K.: Oxford University Press.

    Zuberbuhler, K. 2000a. Causal knowledge of predators behaviour in wild Diana monkeys. Anim.

    Behav. 59:209220.

    Zuberbuhler, K. 2000b. Referential labelling in wild Diana monkeys. Anim. Behav. 59:917927.

    Zuberbuhler, K. 2001. Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbells guenons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

    50:414422.

    Zuberbuhler, K. 2002. A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Anim. Behav.63:293299.

    Zuberbuhler, K. 2005. The phylogenetic roots of language - Evidence from primate communication

    and cognition.Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 14:126130.

    Zuberbuhler, K. 2008. Audience effects. Curr. Biol. 18:R189R190.Zuberbuhler, K., D. L. Cheney, and R. M. Seyfarth. 1999. Conceptual semantics in a nonhuman

    primate. J. Comp. Psychol. 113:3342.

    Zuberbuhler, K., R. Noe, and R. Seyfarth. 1997. Diana monkey long-distance calls: Messages for

    conspecifics and predators. Anim. Behav. 53:589604.

    Primate Vocal Communication / 173

  • 8/10/2019 60742347

    22/22

    Copyright of Human Biology is the property of Wayne State University Press and its content may not be copied

    or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.

    However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.