12
On the Way to Assess Emotions in Animals: Do Lambs ( Ovis aries) Evaluate an Event Through Its Suddenness, Novelty, or Unpredictability? Lara De ´s ire ´ , Isabe lle Veiss ier, Ge ´ rard Despre ´s , and Alain Boiss y Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique Appraisal theories provide a framework that gives insight into emotions and could allow comparisons across species. According to these theories, events are first evaluated on their suddenness, novelty, and unpredictability. The authors examined the ability of lambs ( Ovis aries) to evaluate an event according to these 3 criteria through 3 tests. The lambs responded to suddenness with a startle response coupled with an increase in heart rate and to novelty with an orientation response coupled with an increase of vagal activity. There was no clear evidence that lambs can detect the predictability of an event. Those results support the usefulness of appraisal theories to assess emotion in animals. It is often assumed that animals feel emotions. In fact, animal models are used to study depression and to test anxiolytics. More- over, international conventions as well as regional regulations have been adopted in order to protect animal welfare, on the grounds that they can suffer, for example, see European conventions for the protection of animals kept for farming purposes (Council of Eu- rope, 1992) or exper imen ts (Counc il of Europe, 1998). Emotions, or more generally, mental states, cannot be measured directly. On the basis of the concept of stress, an exten sive list of indica tors of behav ior and physio logy has been set up to distinguis h good from bad welfare. However, these indicators do not allow any inferences to be made about the emotions in animals (Broom, 1998). Knowl- edge of the range of emotions experienced by animals is sparse and, most of the time, one postulates an emotion felt by animals a posteriori, by interpreting the responses observed (De ´sire ´, Boissy, & Veissier , 2002 ). Hence, Dantzer ( 2002) and De ´s ire ´ e t al. (2 002) suggested identifying the emotions of animals on the basis of the cognitive evaluation of situations by the animal itself. They pro- posed orientating its evaluation toward specific evaluation charac- teristics by experimentally manipulating these situations. According to theories of appraisal, which have recently been developed in cognitive psychology, an emotion results from the evaluation of a situation by the individual who is exposed to it (Ellsworth, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Lazarus, 1984; for a review of the diffe rent appraisa l theories, see Scher er, 1999). Among these ap- praisal theories, Scherer (2001) postulates that in humans, situa- tions are evaluated according to a series of six checks, and that the out come of the se che cks det ermine s the nat ure of the ens uing emotio n. The fir st thr ee che cks concer n the rel eva nce of the external eve nt for the indi vidual : (a) the nove lty of the event , which breaks down into three subchecks, in other words, sudden- ness, novelty, and predictability; (b) the intrinsic pleasantness of the event; and (c) the importance of the event for the individual’s objec tives or requir ements (goal– need relev ance ). The fourth check concerns the implication of the event regarding the individ- ual’s needs or expectations. The fifth check concerns the possibil- ity of the individual coping with the event. The last check concerns the compatibility of the event with social or personal standards. The emotion is thus seen as an emergent product of the process of evaluation. Moreover, it is postulated that the checks are organized into a cascade: The evaluation of the implication of the event regarding the individual’s needs is only triggered if the event is perceived as rele vant (i.e., the first three checks) , and the evalua- tion of the possibility of coping with the event is only triggered if the event has implication for the individual’s needs or expectations (Sc her er, 1987). Suc h a seq uentia l organi zat ion of the che cks allows complex emotions to be broken down into simple elements that can be examined for different degrees of difficulty. Apprai sal theories, bas ed on the seq uential organi za tion of checks, are intended to be applicable to different degrees of cog- nitive complexi ty across speci es and individual devel opmen tal stages. Indeed, an increasing number of checks are used as one moves up the evolutionary tree, and the complexity of the checks increases as an organi sm matu res. Thus, appra isal theories are particularly well suited for comparative studies, as they identify which checks are available in the different species and at which devel opmen tal stage s. This issue is impor tant because welf are considerations apply to farm and laboratory animals from different species and, within a given species, to animals of different ages. Acco rding to the sequentia l organi zatio n of the check s, only events that are relevant for the individual will act as elicitors of emotions. The first check is therefore of prime importance. Sud- denne ss, novelty, and unpredicta bility have been recognize d to produce, or at least affect, fear or stress reactions of animals. For instance, sudden or novel events are often used in vario us species Lara De ´si re ´, Isabell e Ve issier, Ge ´r ard Despre ´s, and Alain Boissy, Insti- tut Natio nal de la Rech erche Agron omiq ue—Cl ermon t-Ferr and Theix, Unite ´ de Rech erches sur les Herbivores , Saint -Gene `s-Champ anell e, France. This research was supported by the Conseil Re ´gional d’Auvergne and the Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique. We thank the staff at the Experimental Farm for their care and maintenance of the animals and their assistance during the tests: Herve ´ Tournadre, Jean Teuma, Bernard Malle t, and Pierre Amblard. We than k Eric Delval, Gilles Toporenk o, Ste ´pha ne Andanson, Delphine Montigny, and Fre ´de ´ri ck Bousquie ´ for their assistance during the tests and the analyses. In addition, we thank Hans Erhard and Angela Sibbald for rereading the English. Corres pond ence concerni ng this article shou ld be addre ssed to Lara De ´sire ´, INRA—Clermont-Ferrand Theix, Unite ´ de Recherches sur les Herbivore s, F-6 3122 Saint-Gene `s-Champane lle , France. E-mail: [email protected] Journal of Comparative Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association 2004, Vol. 118, No. 4, 363374 0735-7036/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.118.4.363 363

PsiAnim10

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 1/12

On the Way to Assess Emotions in Animals: Do Lambs ( Ovis aries )Evaluate an Event Through Its Suddenness, Novelty, or Unpredictability?

Lara Desire, Isabelle Veissier, Gerard Despres, and Alain BoissyInstitut National de la Recherche Agronomique

Appraisal theories provide a framework that gives insight into emotions and could allow comparisonsacross species. According to these theories, events are first evaluated on their suddenness, novelty, andunpredictability. The authors examined the ability of lambs ( Ovis aries ) to evaluate an event accordingto these 3 criteria through 3 tests. The lambs responded to suddenness with a startle response coupledwith an increase in heart rate and to novelty with an orientation response coupled with an increase of vagal activity. There was no clear evidence that lambs can detect the predictability of an event. Thoseresults support the usefulness of appraisal theories to assess emotion in animals.

It is often assumed that animals feel emotions. In fact, animal

models are used to study depression and to test anxiolytics. More-over, international conventions as well as regional regulations havebeen adopted in order to protect animal welfare, on the groundsthat they can suffer, for example, see European conventions for theprotection of animals kept for farming purposes (Council of Eu-rope, 1992) or experiments (Council of Europe, 1998). Emotions,or more generally, mental states, cannot be measured directly. Onthe basis of the concept of stress, an extensive list of indicators of behavior and physiology has been set up to distinguish good frombad welfare. However, these indicators do not allow any inferencesto be made about the emotions in animals (Broom, 1998). Knowl-edge of the range of emotions experienced by animals is sparseand, most of the time, one postulates an emotion felt by animals aposteriori, by interpreting the responses observed (Desire, Boissy,& Veissier, 2002). Hence, Dantzer (2002) and Desire et al. (2002)suggested identifying the emotions of animals on the basis of thecognitive evaluation of situations by the animal itself. They pro-posed orientating its evaluation toward specific evaluation charac-teristics by experimentally manipulating these situations.

According to theories of appraisal, which have recently beendeveloped in cognitive psychology, an emotion results from theevaluation of a situation by the individual who is exposed to it(Ellsworth, 1991; Frijda, 1987; Lazarus, 1984; for a review of the

different appraisal theories, see Scherer, 1999). Among these ap-

praisal theories, Scherer (2001) postulates that in humans, situa-tions are evaluated according to a series of six checks, and that theoutcome of these checks determines the nature of the ensuingemotion. The first three checks concern the relevance of theexternal event for the individual: (a) the novelty of the event,which breaks down into three subchecks, in other words, sudden-ness, novelty, and predictability; (b) the intrinsic pleasantness of the event; and (c) the importance of the event for the individual’sobjectives or requirements (goal–need relevance). The fourthcheck concerns the implication of the event regarding the individ-ual’s needs or expectations. The fifth check concerns the possibil-ity of the individual coping with the event. The last check concernsthe compatibility of the event with social or personal standards.The emotion is thus seen as an emergent product of the process of evaluation. Moreover, it is postulated that the checks are organizedinto a cascade: The evaluation of the implication of the eventregarding the individual’s needs is only triggered if the event isperceived as relevant (i.e., the first three checks), and the evalua-tion of the possibility of coping with the event is only triggered if the event has implication for the individual’s needs or expectations(Scherer, 1987). Such a sequential organization of the checksallows complex emotions to be broken down into simple elementsthat can be examined for different degrees of difficulty.

Appraisal theories, based on the sequential organization of checks, are intended to be applicable to different degrees of cog-nitive complexity across species and individual developmentalstages. Indeed, an increasing number of checks are used as onemoves up the evolutionary tree, and the complexity of the checksincreases as an organism matures. Thus, appraisal theories areparticularly well suited for comparative studies, as they identifywhich checks are available in the different species and at whichdevelopmental stages. This issue is important because welfareconsiderations apply to farm and laboratory animals from differentspecies and, within a given species, to animals of different ages.

According to the sequential organization of the checks, onlyevents that are relevant for the individual will act as elicitors of emotions. The first check is therefore of prime importance. Sud-denness, novelty, and unpredictability have been recognized toproduce, or at least affect, fear or stress reactions of animals. Forinstance, sudden or novel events are often used in various species

Lara Desire, Isabelle Veissier, Gerard Despres, and Alain Boissy, Insti-tut National de la Recherche Agronomique—Clermont-Ferrand Theix,Unite de Recherches sur les Herbivores, Saint-Genes-Champanelle,France.

This research was supported by the Conseil Regional d’Auvergne andthe Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique. We thank the staff atthe Experimental Farm for their care and maintenance of the animals andtheir assistance during the tests: Herve Tournadre, Jean Teuma, BernardMallet, and Pierre Amblard. We thank Eric Delval, Gilles Toporenko,Stephane Andanson, Delphine Montigny, and Frederick Bousquie for theirassistance during the tests and the analyses. In addition, we thank HansErhard and Angela Sibbald for rereading the English.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to LaraDesire, INRA—Clermont-Ferrand Theix, Unite de Recherches sur lesHerbivores, F-63122 Saint-Genes-Champanelle, France. E-mail:[email protected]

Journal of Comparative Psychology Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association2004, Vol. 118, No. 4, 363–374 0735-7036/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.118.4.363

363

Page 2: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 2/12

to assess the animals’ reactivity (also called emotionality, fearful-ness, or temperament; Boissy, 1998). Tests for suddenness use thepresentation of a stimulus such as a ball falling suddenly from theceiling in front of the animal (Romeyer & Bouissou, 1992) or ablast of compressed air applied on the muzzle (Boissy & Bouissou,1995). Novelty is always included in the so-called open-field test,

which has been intensively used in birds (Hocking, Channing,Waddington, & Jones, 2001) and mammals (rodents: Archer,1973; carnivores: Pedersen, Moeller, & Jeppesen, 2002; ungulateswith cattle: Boissy & Bouissou, 1995; sheep: Romeyer & Bouis-sou, 1992; pigs: Thodberg, Jensen, & Herskin, 1999; and primates:Watson, Ward, Davis, & Stavisky, 1999). Finally, the unpredict-ability of an aversive stimulus is known to enhance stress re-sponses (Weiss, 1972). For instance, in rats, the corticosteroneresponse to repetitive exposure to a novel cage is more pronouncedwhen the exposures are irregular than when they are regular (Muir& Pfister, 1986). Not only the unpredictability of aversive stimulibut also that of appetitive stimuli can affect the animal. Forinstance, rats present stress-induced analgesia when food deliveryis unpredictable (Tazi, Dantzer, & Le Moal, 1987).

Most of the literature concerns rodents, and little is known aboutfarm ungulates, although this could shed light on the question of animal welfare. From an ecological point of view, suddenness andunpredictability are the key features of a predatory attack, anddomestic ungulates in range environments may still experiencepredation by wild animals or dogs (Shelton & Wade, 1979).Moreover, animal production subjects animals to a range of chal-lenges characterized by novelty, suddenness, and unpredictability(e.g., transport, handling, changes of diet, etc.; Hargreaves &Hutson, 1997).

The observation of behavioral and physiological responses tospecific experimental situations, defined on the basis of a systemof evaluation checks, should help to access the emotional world of

animals. In this article, we report on three tests conducted in lambs.In each of these tests, one of the three subchecks of novelty,namely, suddenness, novelty, or unpredictability, was made moresalient. If lambs are able to perceive an event according to thesethree different subchecks, appraisal theory would predict differentbehavioral and physiological responses depending on which of thesubchecks is the most important. The following questions wereinvestigated.

Do Lambs Perceive an Event Differently According to ItsSuddenness, Novelty, and Unpredictability?

Leventhal and Scherer (1987) proposed that three differentcognitive levels of information processing are implicated in theevaluation process: sensory motor, schematic, and conceptual lev-els. The sensory motor level includes innate expressive-motorprograms, the schematic level requires memory of concrete events,and the conceptual one requires memory of links between concreteevents. For the check of novelty, each subcheck would be pro-cessed at a different cognitive level: suddenness on the sensorymotor level, novelty on the schematic one, and unpredictability onthe conceptual level. It may be that lambs cannot perform allsubchecks of novelty, because they lack the necessary cognitiveabilities. We carried out this study to examine whether all pro-cessing levels are available to lambs. The possibility that lambswill perceive an event in this way would be shown if the behaviorsand/or physiological responses of the lambs differed according to

the situation. Because sheep recognize shapes easily (Kendrick,1994), we exposed them to visual stimuli to study their evaluationprocesses. In the first test, we produced suddenness by making anobject appear rapidly in front of the animal, and the responses werecompared to a control situation in which the object appearedslowly. In the second test, we addressed the question of novelty by

presenting both new and familiar objects. Finally, in the third test,we addressed the question of predictability by giving the animalaccess to food at both regular and irregular times. The behaviorand cardiac activity of the lambs were recorded concomitantly. Wemeasured both heart rate and heart rate variability to assess thesympatho-vagal balance of the autonomous nervous system(Berntson et al., 1997; Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Elec-trophysiology, 1996).

What Are the Specific Behavioral and PhysiologicalResponses Linked to Suddenness, Novelty, or

Unpredictability?

Appraisal theory predicts specific behavioral and physiologicalresponses for each subcheck of novelty. Suddenness (the fastappearance of a stimulus) triggers a startle response and concom-itant heart rate acceleration. Novelty triggers an orientation re-sponse toward the novel event, accompanied by a heart rate de-celeration (Graham, 1979). Unpredictability triggers stressresponses: A lower heart rate variability is therefore expected inlambs given access to food on a random schedule (Porges, 1995).

Method

Experimental Subjects

Twenty-four female lambs ( Ovis aries ) from the INRA401 breed wereexposed to various behavioral tests (see Experimentation period in Proce-dures section below) when they were 3 months of age. They had beenseparated from their dams 18 h after birth and housed in pairs. Separationfrom the dam is usual when ewes have more than two lambs, and theabsence of maternal stimuli may be compensated by the presence of peers(Moberg & Wood, 1982). Three times a day, for 5 days, lambs had beentaught to drink milk and were stroked by the stockperson. During the 6following weeks, 1 day a week they were stroked by the stockperson threetimes in the course of the day. This frequency of contact was higher thanin standard practice in order to minimize the reactivity of animals to thehandling occurring during the subsequent tests. They were weaned at 8weeks of age and then fed hay and concentrates. At the age of 2.5 months,2 weeks prior to the start of experimentation, they were housed together ina large pen called the holding pen (Figure 1). One week prior to the startof experimentation, they were shaved over a 10 cm 10 cm area on theright shoulder and on the left axillary region for later electrocardiogram(ECG) recording (see ECG recording in the Experimental Setup sectionbelow).

Experimental Setup

A testing area consisting of a pretest pen (4 m 2), a test pen (8 m2), anda corridor (3.6 m 2) was constructed next to the holding pen (Figure 1).White lines on the floor divided the pretest pen into nine sectors of equalsize (67 cm 67 cm each) and the test pen into three sectors of equal size(1.3 m 2 m). The pens were separated by solid wooden partitions. Doorsthat could be moved up and down from a remote place allowed passagefrom one pen to another. A red and white traffic cone, 50 cm high, wasfixed in the middle of the holding pen. The lambs were exposed to three

364 DESIRE , VEISSIER, DESPRE S, AND BOISSY

Page 3: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 3/12

experimental situations: a test of suddenness, a test of novelty, and a test of unpredictability (see Experimentation period in the Procedures sectionbelow). During the tests of suddenness and unpredictability, a trough filledwith concentrate was fixed in the test pen at the side opposite the entrance.For the test of suddenness, a device consisting of one rope and two pulleyswas placed behind the trough; it was used by an experimenter outside thetest pen to move a red scarf from behind the trough to 30 cm above it.During the test of novelty, the trough was removed and an object, either atraffic cone (similar to the one fixed in the holding pen) or a tire, was

placed in the middle of the test pen. The tire was black and brown and inan upright vertical position.

Equipment

Behavioral recording. Four cameras (Sony SPT-MC128CE; SonyCorporation, Tokyo, Japan) were used to observe the lambs. They were

connected to a video recorder (Sony SVT-1000P; Sony Corporation, To-kyo, Japan) via a quadravision system (MV25 Multivision Processor,model MX25; Robot Research, San Diego, California) that allowed thesimultaneous recording of the pictures from the four cameras on the sametape. The four cameras were placed, according to the test, in such a way asto observe animals from above or from the sides in both the test and thepretest pens.

ECG recording. Two electrodes were connected to the transmitter of an ambulatory monitoring unit (LifeScope 6 model OEC-6301K, NihonKodhen, Japan). The receptor, which was 2–3 meters outside the test pen,was connected to a PowerPC Apple via a data acquisition system(PowerLab, ADInstruments, Castle Hill, Australia). The rate of acquisitionwas 1,000 points per second. Chart Software (Version 3.6.8, ADInstru-ments, Australia) was used to record the data.

Procedures

Familiarization period. Animals were familiarized to the whole exper-imental setup during the first 2 weeks they were in the holding pen. Everyday during the first week, they had free access to all parts between 8:30a.m. and 5:00 p.m. (all the doors were left open). Every day during thesecond week, the lambs were exposed to the opening of the doors and tothe general procedure of the tests: They were brought into the pretest penfor 30 s, then they were allowed to enter the test pen, where they could eatfor 2 minutes, and finally, they were free to return to the holding penthrough the corridor. On the first 3 days, they were exposed in pairs: On thefirst 2 days, these pairs consisted of animals from the same rearing pair,and on the third day, the pairs consisted of animals from different rearingpairs. On the 4 following days, the lambs were trained alone once a day andwere fitted with an elasticized belt by an experimenter in the pretest pen.

Experimentation period. The experimentations were subsequentlyconducted over 3 consecutive weeks. The test of suddenness and the test of novelty were conducted on separate days and were separated by 1 day. Thetest of unpredictability began 4 days after the test of novelty and wasconducted over 12 consecutive days. Between two tests, the lambs weremoved alone into the test pen once a day without any stimulus (trainingsession). During the entire experimentation period, lambs were fed hay adlibitum in the holding pen, with concentrate given only in the evening andat least a half an hour after the end of the test or training session for thatday. During all test and training sessions, animals were penned up in frontof the pretest pen and therefore had no access to hay.

Test of suddenness. The scarf was moved to its “up” position 30 s afterthe lamb began to eat in the trough, and the lambs were observed for 2 min.The 24 lambs were randomly assigned to one of the two followingtreatments: 12 lambs were exposed to the rapid appearance of the scarf (88cm/s; sudden treatment), and 12 lambs were exposed to the slow appear-ance of the scarf (6 cm/s; slow treatment). Half the animals of eachtreatment were tested in the morning, and the other half were tested in theafternoon.

Test of novelty. The lambs were exposed to the tire or the traffic conefor 100 s after entering the test pen. The 24 lambs were assigned to one of the following treatments: For 12 lambs, the added object was the tire ( noveltreatment), and for the other 12 lambs, it was the familiar traffic cone( familiar treatment). In each treatment, half of the lambs had been exposedto the sudden situation in the previous test, and half had been exposed tothe slow condition. Half of the animals on each treatment were tested in themorning, and the other half were tested in the afternoon.

Test of unpredictability. For the four training sessions before the test of unpredictability, the trough was again placed in the test pen. Then, during

Figure 1. Experimental setup. The thick black lines represent doors; the

solid square represents the position of the trough during tests of suddennessand of unpredictability; the X represents the position of the object duringthe test of novelty; and the shaded areas represent zones in front of thedoors giving access to food or the holding pen in the test of unpredictability.

365ASSESSING EMOTIONS IN SHEEP

Page 4: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 4/12

Page 5: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 5/12

before and after the appearance of the scarf (test of suddenness) or theexposure to the object (test of novelty). For the test of unpredictability, theindices were calculated for time windows of 60 s before the opening of thedoor for Days 1, 2, 5, 8, and 12.

Statistical Analyses

We carried out statistical analyses using the SAS statistical package(Version 8.1, SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina). When the condition of homogeneity of variance could not be assumed from the plot of theresiduals, data were log transformed and used in subsequent analyses if theplot of residuals verified the condition of homogeneity. Significance wasset at the 5% level, and tendency was set at the 10% level.

Test of suddenness. We analyzed the times spent in the differentbehavioral states, except those concerning the behavior toward the scarf,because they could not be observed before its appearance, with a multi-

variate analysis of variance (MANOVA) for repeated measures. The ef-fects assessed were treatment (sudden vs. slow presentation), time (beforevs. after appearance of the object), and their interactions. The times spentlooking at the scarf, sniffing it, and in contact with it were summed up inan overall measure of “exploring the scarf.” We subsequently analyzed itwith an analysis of variance (ANOVA). The effect observed was treatment.We tested the differences in the number of animals that showed a startleresponse with a Fisher’s exact test. Regarding cardiac activity, there was nodifference between the treatment groups before the appearance of the scarf.Therefore, we kept only the values for the 10 s before the appearance of thescarf as the basal value in the analysis. We analyzed the cardiac measureswith a mixed-model ANOVA for repeated measures with a random effectfor the lambs. We used an unstructured model for the covariance. The fixedeffects assessed were treatment (two levels), time (four levels), and theirinteractions. We made post hoc comparisons with least square meansdifferences (LSD).

Figure 2. Test of suddenness—cardiac activity of the lambs exposed to the scarf appearing suddenly (solidlines) versus slowly (dashed lines). A: Mean beat-to-beat intervals (mean RR). B: Root-mean-square of successive beat-to-beat differences (RMSSD). Each cardiac measure is calculated on time windows of 10 s forthe 30 s before and after the appearance of the scarf. Within the sudden treatment, different letters (a, b, and c)indicate significant differences between times, and similar letters with or without a degree symbol indicate onlya tendency for differences ( p .10). ° p .10. ** p .01.

367ASSESSING EMOTIONS IN SHEEP

Page 6: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 6/12

Test of novelty. We analyzed behavioral states with a MANOVA. Theeffect assessed was treatment (novel vs. familiar object). We analyzed thecardiac data in the same way as those from the test of suddenness.

Test of unpredictability. We calculated and subsequently compared themeans of the data collected during the two daily sessions in which lambswere not given access to food with those from the session on the same dayin which the lambs were given access to food. We analyzed sniffing andtime spent in front of the door giving access to food and the index of locomotion with a mixed-model ANOVA with a random effect for thelambs. The fixed effects assessed were treatment (regular vs. randomaccess to food), session (access to food vs. no access to food), day (fivelevels), and their interactions. We made post hoc comparisons using theLSD. We analyzed cardiac data with a mixed-model ANOVA with arandom effect for the lambs. The fixed effects assessed were treatment (twolevels), session (two levels), day (five levels), and their interactions. Posthoc comparisons were made using the LSD.

Results

Test of Suddenness

Four lambs did not complete the test of suddenness because theydid not eat or stay near the trough for 30 s at the end of thefamiliarization period. Because of technical problems, the behav-ioral data for another lamb were lost. Thus, only 20 lambs wereused in the analyses of cardiac activity, and only 19 were used inthose of behavior.

After the appearance of the scarf, the animals modified theirbehavior: They spent less time eating, standing immobile, and inthe zone around the trough, and they spent more time walking,sniffing the trough, moving backward, and in the middle of the testpen (Table 1). No treatment effect (i.e., sudden vs. slow) orTreatment Time interaction was observed on behavior. Notreatment effect was observed on the time spent exploring the scarf (Table 1). Five of the 9 lambs on the sudden treatment were

startled, whereas only 1 out of the 10 on the slow treatment wasstartled (Fisher’s exact test, p .05).

The overall tendency of mean RR to decrease just after theappearance of the scarf was mainly due to the decrease on thesudden treatment (Figure 2A). RMSSD decreased after the appear-ance of the scarf (Figure 2B). This was apparent from 20 s after the

appearance of the scarf onward, irrespective of whether it appearedslowly or suddenly.

Test of Novelty

All lambs entered the test pen within the limit of 20 s, except 1lamb on the novel treatment. Hence, the behavior in the test penwas recorded for only 23 lambs. Overall, there was an effect of treatment on the behavior of the lambs (Table 2). Lambs for whichthe object was novel took longer to enter the test pen and to reachthe zone around the object than those exposed to the familiarobject. They stood immobile, looked at the object, and touched itsooner. They spent more time sniffing and touching the object andless time sniffing the walls, looking at the exit door, or looking atthe environment. Lambs on both treatments spent similar amountsof time looking at the object, standing immobile, and in the zonenear the exit door.

From before to after the opening of the door, mean RR did notvary, whether the object was novel or not (Figure 3A). RMSSDincreased just after the opening of the door in lambs exposed to thenovel object and during the interval 0–10 s (Figure 3B).

Test of Unpredictability

Across days, the lambs on the random treatment tended to crossan increasing number of zones, and on Day 12, they crossed morezones than lambs on the regular treatment (LSD: p .05; Figure

Table 2Test of Novelty: Behaviors of Lambs Toward a Novel Versus a Familiar Object

Behavior

Treatment

Novel (n 11) Familiar ( n 12)

M SE M SE

Latency to enter the test pen (Zone 1) a 12.24 5.9 1.53 0.42General activity

Sniffing the object a 18.1 2.7 7.2 1.3Latency to look at the object a 0.11 0.06 1.82 0.72Looking at the object 13.2 1.9 11.0 1.4Latency of contact with the object 59.2 12.8 89.1 6.2Contact with the object a 19.3 6.7 4.6 3.7Sniffing the wall a 2.7 0.6 7.48 1.78Looking at the exit door a 6.7 1.0 14.4 2.7Looking at the environment 34.8 5.0 47.5 4.1

Locomotor activityLatency to stand immobile a 1.16 0.41 3.77 0.67Standing immobile 63.8 2.9 53.6 5.9

Localization in the test penLatency to enter in the area around the object a 5.71 1.35 1.12 0.11Latency to enter the zone near the exit door a 8.95 3.46 5.01 2.00

Note. Mean time and standard error (in seconds) spent in each behavior are calculated for the 100 s followingthe entrance of the lambs in the test pen. Multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA): F treatment (13, 9) 3.52, p .05.a Log transformed data were used to perform the MANOVA.

368 DESIRE , VEISSIER, DESPRE S, AND BOISSY

Page 7: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 7/12

4A). Lambs on the regular treatment spent more time in front of the door giving access to food and more time sniffing this doorduring sessions in which they were given access to food thanduring sessions in which they were not given access to food(Figure 4B and 4C). This happened from Day 5 on the time spentsniffing the door.

No effect of the treatment was observed on mean RR andRMSSD values (Figure 5A and B). In lambs on the randomtreatment, mean RR was longer (i.e., the heart rate was lower) insessions in which they were given access to food than it was insessions in which they were not given access to food.

Discussion

Specific behavioral and physiological responses were observedin lambs exposed to situations that were sudden and novel and, toa lesser extent, when the situation was unpredictable.

Responses to Suddenness

First of all, when an object was presented to lambs when theywere feeding, they stopped feeding and moved around the pen,whatever the speed of appearance of the object. Their behavior

Figure 3. Test of novelty—cardiac activity of the lambs exposed to a novel (solid lines) versus a familiar(dashed lines) object. A: Mean beat-to-beat intervals (mean RR). B: Root-mean-square of successive beat-to-beatdifferences (RMSSD). Each cardiac measure is calculated on time windows of 10 s for the 30 s before and afterthe opening of the door leading to the test pen where the object was placed. Within treatment, different letters(a, b, c, d, and e) indicate significant differences between times, and similar letters with or without a degree

symbol indicate only a tendency for a differences ( p .10). ° p .10. * p .05.

369ASSESSING EMOTIONS IN SHEEP

Page 8: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 8/12

370 DESIRE , VEISSIER, DESPRE S, AND BOISSY

Page 9: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 9/12

toward the object was independent of its speed of appearance.Moreover, the lambs that were exposed to the rapid appearance of the object were not more reluctant to resume eating than lambsexposed to the slow appearance of the object. The only significantdifference was on the startle response: When the object appearedsuddenly, half of the lambs were startled, whereas very few were

startled when it appeared slowly. A startle response has beenwidely reported in mammals such as rodents, cats, and humans(Yeomans, Li, Scott, & Frankland, 2002). Most of the time, thisresponse is induced by an acoustic stimulus. In our experiment, thestimulus was both visual and acoustic. This may have enhanced theresponse of the lambs, as has been observed in cattle (Lanier,Grandin, Green, Avery, & McGee, 2000). Although it is difficultto conclude from a unique behavioral difference, the consistencyof our results with those already reported in the literature leads usto think that startle is typical of an event perceived as sudden. Inaddition, the heart rate of the lambs increased markedly (i.e., themean RR decreased) in reaction to the sudden presentation of theobject. This increase was rapid and transient, because it wasobserved for only 10 s following the appearance of the object. Thistransient increase in heart rate following sudden events has beenobserved in rats and humans (Graham, 1979). It seems not to bethe result of a metabolic demand linked to physical activity. Theheart rate increased only in lambs exposed to the sudden object,whereas locomotion increased in all lambs. Such an increase incardiac activity, not connected to an increase in locomotion, hasalready been observed in mountain sheep confronted with disturb-ing events such as the passage of a motor vehicle, the approach of a person, a coyote, or a dog, or a conspecific trotting or runningrapidly toward the subject (MacArthur, Geist, & Johnston, 1982;MacArthur, Johnston, & Geist, 1979). An increase in heart ratethat is independent of somatic activity has also been reported inhumans (Obrist, 1976).

During the increase in heart rate, there was no modification of its variability, as seen in the stability of RMSSD. RMSSD reflectsvagal activity (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiologyand the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology,1996; for data on lambs, see Despres, Boissy, Desire, Le Neindre,& Veissier, 2003). Hence, in our experiment, the increase in heartrate was not due to a lower activity of the parasympathetic branchof the autonomous nervous system. It may have resulted from anenhanced activity of the sympathetic branch. However, to date,there is no reliable heart rate variability indicator of activity of thesympathetic branch that could be used to confirm that this occursin sheep (Despres et al., 2003). Such an increase in heart rate dueto sympathetic activation in the absence of a decrease of parasym-pathetic activity has been reported in rats exposed to an abrupt andintense acoustic stimulus (Berntson, Boysen, & Cacioppo, 1992).Therefore, lambs, like other mammals, react to sudden events witha startle response and a rapid, large, and transient increase in heartrate that is likely to be due to an enhanced sympathetic tone. Thisis in accordance with the characteristics of the startle reflex pro-

posed by Graham (1979), who considered it as an interruptedsystem. Others considered this reflex mainly as a protective re-sponse to predation and only secondarily as a preparation foraction (Yeomans et al., 2002).

Responses to Novelty

In our study, novelty evoked responses different from thoseproduced by suddenness. When a novel object was presented tolambs, they looked at it more rapidly than they did a familiarobject. This result is consistent with previous findings showingthat stimuli carrying information trigger an orienting reflex(Graham, 1979; Turpin, 1986). In our experiment, lambs alsoinvestigated the novel object longer than they investigated thefamiliar one (i.e., they sniffed it longer), but they were morereluctant to touch it. Hence novelty seems to have stimulated bothexploration and neophobia. Furthermore, the heart rates of thelambs were more variable in the presence of the novel object. Inchimpanzees, rats, and humans, the orienting reflex is accompa-nied by a heart rate deceleration of vagal origin (Berntson et al.,1992). In our study, the increased heart rate variability in thepresence of the novel object also suggests such an increase in vagalactivity. However, this enhanced vagal activity was not accompa-nied by bradycardia. The increase in vagal activity may have beencompensated for by an increase in the activity of the sympatheticbranch. In fact, such a double activation has been reported in ratsexposed to novel noises of low intensities (Berntson et al., 1992).Therefore, lambs, like other mammals, react to a novel event withorientation and neophobia responses, and this might be accompa-nied by a concomitant activation of the two branches of theautonomous nervous system.

Responses to Unpredictability

We found behavioral evidence that lambs perceive the unpre-dictability of the outcome of a situation. The lambs that had regularaccess to the reward pen—where food was available—stayed infront of its door or sniffed it longer during sessions that were goingto be rewarded compared with sessions that were going to beunrewarded, and longer than lambs given irregular access to thereward. This was mainly observed from the fifth session for thetime spent sniffing the door, that is, after a visual cue had beenadded. Hence one might conclude that regular timing is not enoughfor lambs to predict their environment and that visual cues arenecessary. If lambs can evaluate the predictability of a signaledevent but not of a regular unsignaled event, signaled farmingprocedures may trigger different emotions rather than regular ones.On the other hand, the visual cue that was present only duringrewarded sessions may have attracted the lambs’ attention andstimulated their activity. If this was the case, lambs may not havemodified their behavior because they knew the outcome of the test,but in response to the visual stimulus. However, the locomotor

Figure 4 (opposite). Test of unpredictability—behaviors of the lambs in the pretest pen given access to the testpen on a regular (REG) versus a random (RAN) schedule when the door giving access to food was about to beopened or was not about to be opened (food vs. holding pen) for Days 1, 2, 5, 8, and 12. A: Number of squarescrossed. B: Time spent in front of the door giving access to food. C: Time spent sniffing the door giving accessto food. ° p .10. * p .05. ** p .01. *** p .001.

371ASSESSING EMOTIONS IN SHEEP

Page 10: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 10/12

activity of the lambs that could not predict the occurrence of thereward increased progressively across sessions, whereas it re-mained constant in the lambs that were given regular rewards withno difference when the visual cue was added. This increasedlocomotion could reflect a hesitation between the two doors bylambs that could not predict which door would be opened. Theheart rate of the lambs also differed between the two treatments. Inlambs that could predict the access to the food reward, we found

no difference between rewarded and unrewarded sessions. In con-trast, lambs that could not predict had a higher heart rate duringunrewarded sessions than during rewarded ones, even though theseanimals could not know which door would be opened. Hence thisresult must be taken as an artifact. Therefore, locomotor activityprovided the only behavioral evidence that lambs learned whetherthey could predict the outcome of the test, because the otherbehaviors (time spent in front of the door giving access to the food

Figure 5. Test of unpredictability—cardiac activity of the lambs in the pretest pen given access to food on a

regular (REG) versus a random (RAN) schedule when the door giving access to food was about to be openedor was not about to be opened (food vs. holding pen) for Days 1, 2, 5, 8, and 12. A: Mean beat-to-beat intervals(mean RR). B: Root-mean-square of successive beat-to-beat differences (RMSSD). Each cardiac measure wascalculated on time windows of 60 s. * p .05. ** p .01. *** p .001.

372 DESIRE , VEISSIER, DESPRE S, AND BOISSY

Page 11: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 11/12

and sniffing this door) may be explained as their simply payingattention to an interesting feature of their environment. Moreover,there was no evidence that unpredictability triggered any specificphysiological responses such as the hypothesized decrease in para-sympathetic activity.

Different Levels of Evaluation

Our results suggest that in lambs, as in other animals, sudden-ness leads to a startle response, whereas novelty leads to anorienting response. These behavioral responses are accompaniedby different physiological responses: Suddenness leads to a tran-sient tachycardia—probably due to high sympathetic activity—andnovelty leads to an increase in heart rate variability—probably dueto both high vagal and sympathetic activities. Hence, not onlybehavioral responses but also physiological ones seem to be spe-cific to these two subchecks. This is in accordance with thespecificity of behavioral and physiological responses for eachevaluation check proposed by Scherer (1987). It challenges the

nonspecificity of physiological responses proposed by Dantzer andMormede (1983). Although lambs seem to evaluate an event on itsunpredictability, as seen in the increase in locomotor activity of thelambs that could not predict access to food, no concomitant spe-cific physiological response was observed. The increase in loco-motor activity was clear only on Day 12, and the other behaviors,because of the methods used, could not be clearly linked to thepossibility that the lambs could predict which door would beopened. Thus, further experiments are needed to ascertain whetherlambs can evaluate an event by its predictability.

In this study, lambs clearly evaluated events on their suddennessand novelty. Regarding the paradigm used for studying the rele-vance of the unpredictability of an event, the slowness of lambs tolearn which door would be opened may be explained by theirprevious experience. Before the test of unpredictability, lambswere trained to access food each time they entered the pretest pen.Thus, when lambs were given the tests of unpredictability, theresponses they had already acquired had first to become extinctand may have delayed their learning that access to food wouldoccur only on the first, second, or third time of access to the pretestpen. Moreover, learning from three sessions a day that are notsuccessive may be more difficult than learning from three succes-sive sessions. Finally, predictability was first presented throughtiming only and was only later extended to include a visual cue tosignal access to the food, which may have made it easier and fasterto learn. Thus, further studies with a simpler schedule and notconfounded by previous experience are needed to ascertain thatlambs can evaluate an event by its predictability and to identify thespecific responses linked to such an event. Nevertheless, regardingthe three different levels of information processing (Scherer,2001), the evaluation of suddenness is processed on the sensori-motor level, the evaluation of novelty is processed on the sche-matic level, because it requires a schema matching, and the eval-uation of unpredictability is processed on the conceptual level,because it is based on complex estimates of the probability of theoccurrence of a stimulus. This hierarchy of processing levels isconsidered to be reflected in phylogeny and ontogeny (Leventhal& Scherer, 1987). It may be that the conceptual level is notavailable to lambs. This would explain why we found no clearevidence for specific responses to unpredictability.

Because animal welfare involves the subjective feelings of animals (Dawkins, 1990), any knowledge about the range of emotions that an individual of a given species can experience mayhelp to improve their welfare. Thus, knowing more about theappraisal abilities of lambs and about which evaluation checks areused in a given situation will make it possible to hypothesize

which kind of emotion lambs will experience. Furthermore, adescription of farming procedures according to the evaluation theanimals may trigger could help assess which procedures may resultin negative or positive emotions. Farm animals can learn temporaland instrumental contingencies (Nicol, 1996), which are a goodindex of their ability to anticipate and to access the conceptuallevel of processing. Thus, there is a theoretical possibility thatlambs can evaluate an event by its unpredictability, even thoughwe failed to find clear evidence for this in the current study.

Conclusion

As postulated by appraisal theories in cognitive psychology, theemotion triggered by an event depends on the result of the eval-uation of that event. Such theories provide a useful framework forthe study of the nature of subjective experiences in animals thatcould help us to understand their welfare requirements. Weshowed in lambs that a sudden event triggered a startle response,which was accompanied by a transient increase in heart rate thatwas probably of sympathetic origin, whereas a novel event inducedan orientation response accompanied by increased activity of theparasympathetic branch. Weak evidence supported the possibilitythat lambs evaluate predictability, but this result needs to bevalidated in further experiments. Thus, it seems that the relevanceof an external event can be evaluated by lambs at least in terms of suddenness and novelty, and these two elementary appraisalchecks are perceived differently. This shows that lambs are not

passive in their environment and can be attentive to slight modi-fication of it according to their cognitive abilities. Our results withsheep support the theories of appraisal in emotions, at least re-garding simple checks. They confirm that these theories provide abasic framework for the study of emotions in a large range of animals and that animal models can be used to study aspects of human emotions, through observable elements rather than verbalreports. It remains to be shown that lambs can access the highersteps of the evaluation process, such as the assessment of theimplication of an event in relation to the individual’s needs as wellas the assessment of the possibility of coping with the event. Oncethe elementary evaluation checks that are used by lambs areidentified, it will be possible to carry out objective studies todetermine which emotions are experienced by animals by usingparticular combinations of these checks.

References

Archer, J. (1973). Tests for emotionality in rats and mice: A review. Animal Behaviour, 21, 205–235.

Berntson, G. G., Bigger, J. T. J., Eckberg, D. L., Grossman, P., Kaufmann,P. G., Malik, M., et al. (1997). Heart rate variability: Origins, methods,and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology, 34, 623–648.

Berntson, G. G., Boysen, S. T., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1992). Cardiac orientingand defensive responses: Potential origins in autonomic space. In B. A.Campbell, H. Hayne, & R. Richardson (Eds.), Attention and information processing in infants and adults. Perspectives from human and animalresearch (pp. 163–200). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

373ASSESSING EMOTIONS IN SHEEP

Page 12: PsiAnim10

8/12/2019 PsiAnim10

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/psianim10 12/12

Boissy, A. (1998). Fear and fearfulness in determining behavior. In T.Grandin (Ed.), Genetics and the behavior of domestic animals (pp.67–111). New York: Academic Press.

Boissy, A., & Bouissou, M.-F. (1995). Assessment of individual differ-ences in behavioural reactions of heifers exposed to various fear-eliciting situations. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 46, 17–31.

Broom, D. M. (1998). Welfare, stress, and the evolution of feelings. In P.

Slater, A. Møller, & M. Manfred (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (pp. 371–403). New York: Academic Press.

Council of Europe. (1992). Protocol of amendment to the European Con-vention for the protection of animals kept for farming purposes. Euro- pean Treaty Series, 145.

Council of Europe. (1998). Protocol of amendment to the European Con-vention for the protection of vertebrate animals used for experimentaland other purposes. European Treaty Series, 170.

Dantzer, R. (2002). Can farm animal welfare be understood without takinginto account the issues of emotion and cognition? Journal of AnimalScience, 80, E1–E9.

Dantzer, R., & Mormede, P. (1983). Stress in farm animals: A need forreevaluation. Journal of Animal Science, 57, 6–18.

Dawkins, M. S. (1990). From an animal’s point of view: Motivation,fitness, and animal welfare. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 1–61.

Desire, L., Boissy, A., & Veissier, I. (2002). Emotions in farm animals: Anew approach to animal welfare in applied ethology. Behavioural Pro-cesses, 60, 165–180.

Despres, G., Boissy, A., Desire, L., Le Neindre, P., & Veissier, I. (2003).Validation of the measure of sympatho-vagal effect in lambs throughautonomic blockades and heart rate variability indexes. Journal of An-imal and Veterinary Advances, 2, 615–619.

Ellsworth, P. C. (1991). Some implications of cognitive appraisal theoriesof emotion. In K. T. Strongman (Ed.), International review of studies onemotion (Vol. 1., pp. 143–160). Chichester, England: Wiley.

Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotion, cognitive structure, and action tendency.Cognition and Emotion, 1, 115–143.

Graham, F. K. (1979). Distinguishing among orienting, defensive, andstartle reflexes. In H. D. Kimmel, E. H. van Olst, & J. F. Orlebeke (Eds.),The orienting reflex in humans (pp. 137–167). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hargreaves, A. L., & Hutson, G. D. (1997). Handling systems for sheep. Livestock Production Science, 49, 121–138.

Hocking, P. M., Channing, C. E., Waddington, D., & Jones, R. B. (2001).Age-related changes in fear, sociality and pecking behaviours in twostrains of laying hen. British Poultry Science, 42, 414–423.

Kendrick, K. M. (1994). Neurobiological correlates of visual and olfactoryrecognition in sheep. Behavioural Processes, 33, 89–112.

Lanier, J. L., Grandin, T., Green, R. D., Avery, D., & McGee, K. (2000).The relationship between reaction to sudden, intermittent movementsand sounds and temperament. Journal of Animal Science, 78, 1467–1474.

Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. American Psycholo-gist, 39, 124–129.

Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. R. (1987). The relationship of emotion to

cognition: A functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognitionand Emotion, 1, 3–28.MacArthur, R. A., Geist, V., & Johnston, R. H. (1982). Physiological

correlates of social behaviour in bighorn sheep: A field study usingelectrocardiogram telemetry. Journal of Zoology, 196, 401–415.

MacArthur, R. A., Johnston, R. H., & Geist, V. (1979). Factors influencingheart rate in free-ranging bighorn sheep: A physiological approach to the

study of wildlife harassment. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 57, 2010–2021.

Moberg, G. P., & Wood, V. A. (1982). Effect of differential rearing on thebehavioral and adrenocortical response of lambs to a novel environment. Applied Animal Ethology, 8, 269–279.

Muir, J. L., & Pfister, H. P. (1986). Corticosterone and prolactin responsesto predictable and unpredictable novelty stress in rats. Physiology &

Behavior, 37, 285–288.Nicol, C. J. (1996). Farm animal cognition. Animal Science, 62, 375–391.Obrist, P. A. (1976). The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction—as it

appears today. Psychophysiology, 13, 95–107.Pedersen, V., Moeller, N. H., & Jeppesen, L. L. (2002). Behavioural and

physiological effects of post-weaning handling and access to shelters infarmed blue foxes ( Alopex lagopus ). Applied Animal Behaviour Science,77, 139–154.

Porges, S. W. (1995). Cardiac vagal tone: A physiological index of stress. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 19, 225–233.

Romeyer, A., & Bouissou, M.-F. (1992). Assessment of fear reactions indomestic sheep, and influence of breed and rearing conditions. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 34, 93–119.

Scherer, K. R. (1987). Toward a dynamic theory of emotion: The compo-nent process model of affective states. Geneva Studies in Emotion and Communication, 1, 1–98.

Scherer, K. R. (1999). Appraisal theory. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power(Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 637–663). Chichester,England: Wiley.

Scherer, K. R. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multi-levelsequential checking. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 92–120). New York: Oxford University Press.

Shelton, M., & Wade, D. (1979). Predatory losses: A serious livestock problem. Animal Industry Today, 2, 4–9.

Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North AmericanSociety of Pacing and Electrophysiology. (1996). Heart rate variability:Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use.Circulation, 93, 1043–1065.

Tazi, A., Dantzer, R., & Le Moal, M. (1987). Prediction and control of foodrewards modulate endogenous pain inhibitory systems. Behavioural Brain Research, 23, 197–204.

Thodberg, K., Jensen, K. H., & Herskin, M. S. (1999). A general reactionpattern across situations in prepubertal gilts. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 63, 103–119.

Turpin, G. (1986). Effects of stimulus intensity on autonomic responding:The problem of differentiating orienting and defense reflexes. Psycho- physiology, 23, 1–14.

Watson, S. L., Ward, J. P., Davis, K. B., & Stavisky, R. C. (1999).Scent-marking and cortisol response in the small-eared bushbaby ( Otole-mur garnettii ). Physiology & Behavior, 66, 695–699.

Weiss, J. M. (1972). Psychological factors in stress and disease. Scientific American, 226, 104–113.

Yeomans, J. S., Li, L., Scott, B. W., & Frankland, P. W. (2002). Tactile,

acoustic and vestibular systems sum to elicit the startle reflex. Neuro-science and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 1–11.

Received August 13, 2003Revision received February 24, 2004

Accepted February 29, 2004

374 DESIRE , VEISSIER, DESPRE S, AND BOISSY